Abstract
Belief in life after death is among the oldest forms of spiritual belief, found in nearly every world civilization and religion. While several national surveys highlight differences in afterlife beliefs across countries, none have explored the sociodemographic factors associated with these beliefs. Using data from Wave 1 of the Global Flourishing Study (N = 202,898), weighted to be nationally representative, we estimated the proportion of people affirming belief in an afterlife in 22 countries. Primary analyses with demographic variables were conducted separately by country and then pooled using meta-analytic techniques. We examined variations in afterlife belief across nine sociodemographic characteristics: age, gender, marital status, employment status, education level, immigration status, frequency of religious service attendance, religious affiliation, and racial/ethnic identity. The overall proportion of the populations believing in life after death ranged from 95% in Indonesia to 21% in Japan. Meta-analytic results indicated cross-national heterogeneity across all sociodemographic categories, though the degree of variation differed. Random effects meta-analytic results highlighted religious service attendance as a key factor, with the highest belief in the afterlife observed among those attending services at least once per week. These findings provide a foundation for understanding population-level beliefs in the afterlife and continued exploration of their complexity across different contexts.
Similar content being viewed by others
Belief in life after death: Sociodemographic Variations across 22 Countries
Belief in life after death stands as one of the oldest manifestations of spiritual belief, finding expressions in almost every world civilization1 and established religion2. Closely connected with the general agreement among world religions on an immaterial aspect of human existence3, the notion that human life transcends corporeal demise is central to much religious belief and practice. Thus, the examination of afterlife beliefs is essential for a comprehensive understanding of religious beliefs.
Afterlife beliefs wield profound influence on values, worldviews, and priorities4. Afterlife beliefs can lead to a wide array of actions, from virtuous to hazardous. For example, fear of punishment and hope for a better afterlife may foster ethical conduct and benevolent deeds in the present realm5, or religious adherents may be swayed by the promise of posthumous rewards6. At the same time, afterlife beliefs may inform a distorted perception of gains from costly and immoral actions, such as those associated with suicide terrorism7. Afterlife beliefs may also be used as coping strategy8, and is associated with lower anxiety, depression, and cynicism about the world9,10.
Cultural and sociodemographic variations
Several national surveys have estimated the prevalence of belief in life after death. According to a Gallup11 survey of 57,768 adults in 61 countries, 57% think that there is a life after death. Although not explicitly examined in the Gallup study, this belief appears closely intertwined with religious affiliation. Individuals in highly religious nations, such as Indonesia, Libya, Pakistan, Sierra Leone, and Senegal exhibit a higher propensity to endorse beliefs in life after death, whereas skepticism tends to prevail among residents of Japan, Vietnam, and several European Union countries where religious affiliation is less widespread.
Beyond religious affiliation, afterlife beliefs may vary across other sociodemographic factors. For instance, women consistently report higher levels of religious belief, including afterlife beliefs12. Age also emerges as a potentially influential factor, with spiritual concerns often intensifying with advancing age, a phenomenon referred to as gerotranscendence13. However, even younger individuals tend to report belief in life after death, even while many of their perspectives (particularly in secular contexts) are shaped by current societal trends emphasizing spirituality over traditional religiosity14,15. This is exemplified by data from the UK in the World Values Survey16, where despite a decline in belief in God over the past four decades, belief in life after death remains prevalent, with 46% of respondents expressing such convictions.
The influence of education on afterlife beliefs is multifaceted. While higher education may attenuate adherence to exclusivist religious doctrines, it often boosts a preference for institutionalized religion17. The relationship between cognitive complexity and religiosity is nuanced, as individuals operate within both reflective and non-reflective belief modes18. Moreover, empirical evidence from a comprehensive 14-study experimental research project demonstrates that analytical thinking does not inherently conflict with religious commitment19.
The Present study
Variations in beliefs about life after death at the country level may be influenced by sociodemographic factors, although this aspect remains largely unexplored in existing literature. To help make strides toward addressing this gap, this study aims to investigate associations between demographic variables and afterlife beliefs within and across nations. Drawing on nationally representative data from 22 countries, our study pursues two main objectives: (1) to examine the distribution of afterlife beliefs within each cultural context and (2) to assess potential sociodemographic disparities in afterlife beliefs within and across these countries.
As an initial step, we provide a descriptive analysis of sociodemographic characteristics such as age, gender, marital status, employment status, educational attainment, frequency of religious service attendance, and immigration status. Although our primary focus lies in exploring the distribution of afterlife beliefs across countries, we anticipate encountering meaningful cross-national variation in these patterns. Building upon prior research11, we hypothesize that sociodemographic factors will play a significant role in shaping afterlife beliefs across all countries. In particular, it is expected that religious service attendance will demonstrate the strongest association with afterlife beliefs. While our primary emphasis rests on investigating sociodemographic disparities in afterlife beliefs across diverse cultural contexts, we also anticipate observing some degree of cross-national variation in these sociodemographic differences. This expectation stems from the unique sociocultural landscapes present in each country20,21, which may influence the manifestation and interpretation of afterlife beliefs among different demographic groups.
Results
The sociodemographic characteristics of the total sample are detailed in Table 1. Countries with the largest representation were the United States (19%), Japan (10%), and Sweden (7.4%), while the smallest samples came from Turkey (0.7%), South Africa (1.3%), and Hong Kong (1.5%). The most represented age group was 30–39 years (20%), followed by 40–49 years (17%) and 50–59 years (16%). Gender distribution was nearly equal, with 49% male and 51% female. A majority of participants were married (53%), employed (57%), had 9–15 years of education (57%) and were born in the country where data were collected (94%). Regarding religious service attendance, 32% attended at least once a week, whereas 37% never attended.
Nationally representative sociodemographic characteristics of each country are reported in Supplemental Tables S1a to S22a, with some variability observed across the countries. For example, the percentage of the population who never attend any religious services annually is notably higher in Japan (77%), Australia (67%), and Sweden (66%) compared to Nigeria (1.1%), Kenya (4.1%), and Indonesia (4.8%).
Table 2 provides the ordered proportions of countries of people who believe in life after death. The countries with the highest proportions were Indonesia (0.95), Nigeria (0.73), and Kenya (0.72), while the lowest were Japan (0.21), Sweden (0.29), and Germany (0.34). These trends may be influenced by factors such as secularization, industrialization, and demographic shifts — countries with lower belief proportions tend to be more industrialized with aging populations, while those with higher proportions often exhibit a youth bulge and rapid urbanization.
The lower proportions of endorsement may be partly attributed to the survey offering an ‘Unsure’ response, which some participants selected as an alternative to ‘Yes.’ Supplement Table 24 provides the ordered proportions analogous to Table 2 but dichotomizes responses as Yes/Unsure versus No. Overall, the proportions of people endorsing either ‘Yes’ or ‘Unsure’ for belief in the afterlife were significantly higher, ranging from 0.98 to 0.57. The countries with the highest proportions were Indonesia (0.98) and the Philippines (0.92), while India (0.57) and Sweden (0.63) had the lowest. Supplement Table 25 shows the ordered proportions of those who responded ‘Unsure.’ Japan had the highest proportion of ‘Unsure’ responses (0.47), despite having the lowest proportion of ‘Yes’ responses among all countries. These variations and uncertainties underscore the complexity of endorsing belief in the afterlife across these countries.
Sociodemographic differences in afterlife belief
Table 3 presents the results of random effects meta-analyses. The overall meta-analytic mean was 0.54. Across countries, 95% confidence intervals (CIs) for the proportions revealed significant differences between categories of religious service attendance. Specifically, belief in the afterlife was lower among those who never attend religious services (0.38, 95% CI: 0.30, 0.46) or attend a few times a year (0.53, 95% CI: 0.45, 0.61) compared to those who attend weekly (0.73, 95% CI: 0.66, 0.78) or more frequently (0.78, 95% CI: 0.71, 0.84). On average across countries, the meta-analytic mean proportion were similar across categories of gender, marital status, employment, education, or immigration status across countries. For each of these sociodemographic characteristics, the meta-analytic average proportion was commonly between 0.50 and 0.60 with a few exceptions such as those reporting to be in a domestic partnership (43%). The pattern with age was relatively constant, with a slight inverted-U-shaped pattern from 53% age 18–24, to 57% age 50–59, down to 51% age 70–79, but then those aged 80 + reportedly notably higher at 75%.
Tau values indicate cross-country variation in afterlife belief across sociodemographic categories, although variation was greater for some categories than others. For example, tau was higher for the 80 or older age category (τ = 0.55) compared to other age groups (τ = 0.20–0.22), suggesting greater cross-national variability in afterlife belief within this older age group; however, the observed sample size for this group was also quite small in several countries leading to large uncertainty in these estimates.
All global p-values in Table 3 passed the Bonferroni-corrected significance threshold, indicating differences for each sociodemographic variable in at least one country. Country-specific results for sociodemographic differences in afterlife belief are detailed in Supplemental Tables S1b–S22b, with accompanying forest plots ranking values by country for each sociodemographic category (see Supplemental Figures S1–S34), and the difference in proportions between each category and a refence category (see Supplemental Figures S35–S115).
We identified instances where country-specific differences between sociodemographic categories emerged that were not observed in the random effects meta-analyses. Pooled meta-analytic results suggested that afterlife belief were slightly increasing with age through age 50–59. However, in many countries young adults reported higher belief than middle aged and older adults (e.g., Japan, Germany). As noted above, in the oldest group (80+) there was a markedly higher belief (see Supplemental Figure S8). Although meta-analytic results did not suggest much difference in afterlife belief by gender, country-specific variations were evident. For example, women exhibited much higher belief in Australia, Poland, and Sweden, whereas men had significantly higher belief in Egypt and Tanzania. On average, educational level was not strongly associated with belief when results were pooled across countries; however, there were notable differences between education groups in some countries. More years of education were linked to higher belief in some countries such as Brazil, Egypt, Indonesia, Kenya, Mexico, the Philippines, Tanzania, and the United Kingdom. In contrast, an inverse correlation was observed in Australia, India, Israel, South Africa, Spain, Sweden, Turkey, and the United States.
Country-specific variation in afterlife belief based on religious affiliation and racial/ethnic identity (when available) was also estimated (see Supplemental Tables S1b–S22b). These variables were not included in the meta-analysis due to differing assessments across countries. However, the results do provide some evidence of meaningful differences in certain countries. For example, in a highly religious country like South Africa (predominantly Christian), the proportion of individuals endorsing afterlife belief among those with no religion was similar to that of Christians and other religious groups (see Supplemental Table S16b). In contrast, in a more secular country like Sweden (see Supplemental Table S18b), belief endorsement among the nonreligious was significantly lower than among the religious. Regarding racial differences belief was, for example, significantly lower among those who self-identified as White compared to other racial/ethnic minority groups in the United Kingdom (see Supplemental Table S21b). When we repeated the meta-analyses using a population-weighted approach, where each country’s results were weighted by its 2023 population size, the pattern of results was largely consistent with those from the random-effects meta-analysis (see Supplemental Table S23). Some slight differences emerged, which may be due in part to the substantial weight given to India.
Discussion
In this study, we utilized multinational data from wave 1 of the Global Flourishing Study (GFS) to explore the distribution of afterlife belief across 22 geographically and culturally diverse countries. Three main findings emerged: (1) evidence of cross-national variation in afterlife belief, (2) sociodemographic differences across countries, and (3) cross-national variation in sociodemographic differences in afterlife belief.
Our results reveal substantial country-specific variation in afterlife belief. For example, Indonesia, Nigeria, and Kenya had the highest proportions of people believing in life after death, whereas Japan, Sweden, and Germany had the lowest. Perhaps unsurprisingly, religious involvement may be the most significant of several factors potentially contributing to this heterogeneity. The countries with the highest belief in afterlife are predominantly religious societies where active religious participation is common: Indonesia (92% affiliated with Islam, 4.8% never attend religious services), Nigeria (51% Christianity, 48% Islam, 1.1% never attend religious services), and Kenya (91% Christianity, 4.1% never attend religious services). Conversely, countries with lower belief rates are more secular, such as Japan (61% with no religion, 77% never attend religious services), or report greater identification with religion than religious attendance, such as Sweden (55% Christianity, 38% no religion, 66% never attend religious services) and Germany (53% Christianity, 40% no religion, 62% never attend religious services).
When isolating the “Unsure” responses, we found that India had the lowest percentage (57%) of combined Yes/Unsure responses. This figure aligns precisely with data from the World Values Survey 2017–2022 wave for the Yes/Don’t Know category. One potential contributor to India’s lower belief in the afterlife is its lower rate of religious attendance. Despite being one of the most religious countries, with 81% of the sample identifying as Hindu, 12% as Muslim, and only 0.1% reporting no religion, only 48% of the Indian sample attended religious services at least once a week. Hinduism emphasizes different aspects of religious observance, such as adherence to norms of purity, rather than compulsory attendance at temples.
The high rate of “Unsure” responses in Japan may reflect the country’s complex religious landscape and the unique ways religion is perceived and practiced there. Although Japan is a highly secular country, Shinto and Buddhist traditions remain deeply embedded in its society. Many Japanese individuals participate in religious rituals and ancestor worship, reflecting an integration of spiritual practices into daily life rather than organized religious observance22.
Sociodemographic differences in afterlife belief
Using a random-effects meta-analysis, we examined the proportion of people believing in the afterlife across different demographic groups in 22 countries. We found that belief was highest among those who attend religious services at least once per week and lowest among those who never attend. This difference may be influenced by the degree of religiosity and the dominant religious affiliation in these countries. In countries with a dominant religion (where over 40% of the population adheres to one religion), such as Christianity in Argentina, Australia, Germany, Kenya, Mexico, the Philippines, Poland, Spain, and the US; Islam in Egypt, Indonesia, and Turkey; Judaism in Israel; and Hinduism in India, the dominant religion tends to drive belief in an afterlife. South Africa appears to be an exception to this pattern, as only about 52% of Christians (53% endorsed ‘Yes’ in the general population, and 18% endorsed ‘Unsure’) endorse afterlife belief, despite the country being over 80% Christian. The unique landscape of South African Christianity may be shaped by a complex historical, cultural, and social context, including the influence of African spirituality, Pentecostalism, and the legacy of apartheid.
Some countries, like Nigeria and Tanzania, have similar proportions of Christians and Muslims, with both religions showing strong endorsement of afterlife belief, although Muslims in Tanzania are more likely to believe in the afterlife than Christians. In more secular countries, where no single religion represents more than 40% of the population and the majority is non-religious, such as Hong Kong (50% no religion followed by 25% Christianity) and Japan (61% no religion followed by 33% Buddhism), being religious is still associated with higher belief in the afterlife. In countries with a large Christian population alongside a substantial non-religious group (over 30%), like Sweden and the UK, Christian belief in the afterlife is lower (below 50% of the Christian population).
These findings should be considered in light of the complex understandings of life and the afterlife, which vary significantly across different religious traditions. For instance, in Muslim contexts, heaven (Jannah) is envisioned as a paradise of joy and delights, with levels of divine pleasure based on one’s righteousness23. In Christian contexts, heaven is often viewed as God’s dwelling place, where people from all nations gather to worship him—a vision of communal worship (e.g. expressed famously in St. Augustine’s account of the beatific vision)—but there is significant variation among Christians concerning the role of individual righteousness determining one’s destiny after death. In Dharmic traditions (e.g., Buddhism and Hinduism), karma plays a central role in afterlife beliefs, with present actions influencing future cycles of death and rebirth. Adding to this complexity, even within traditions that emphasize eternal reward and punishment, there is variability. For example, among U.S. adults, 71% believe in heaven, but only 61% believe in hell24. These diverse motivations and expectations for the afterlife suggest that the prevalence of different afterlife beliefs may be influenced by complex relationships between these beliefs and other factors, and future research on the effects of afterlife beliefs can account for differences between specific beliefs.
Afterlife belief can also form experiences and perceptions of ultimate meaning, purpose, and transcendence that is not explicitly connected with organized religious belief and practice. These enhanced perceptions of meaning and purpose may have to do with aging and the life review process, by which older people reflect on their lives, resolve conflicts, and prepare for death25. In the aggregated analysis, we found that belief in the afterlife was highest among oldest age group (80+), although this varied significantly across countries. For example, countries like Indonesia, Egypt, and Nigeria show high proportions of afterlife belief among those over 60, while in Japan, Sweden, and Germany, the proportion of older individuals who believe in the afterlife tends to be less. This variation may, again, be influenced by the different religious landscapes in these countries, and/or the age-period-cohort effects26. We anticipate that data collected in the coming years of the Global Flourishing Study will further clarify the roles of age and religion in shaping afterlife beliefs. While anyone may hold beliefs about the afterlife, proximity to the end of life may generally heighten such considerations.
A major strength of this study is the use of large, nationally representative samples to document belief in afterlife across multiple countries. By estimating belief in afterlife at both national and subpopulation levels, this study lays the groundwork for a more focused study of this important human universal belief. However, there are some methodological limitations. First, afterlife belief was assessed with a single item, which may capture the essence of the construct but overlook its complexity and how people understand life after death. Second, while wave 1 of the GFS includes 22 geographically and culturally diverse countries, it does not represent all global contexts (e.g., mainland China), so caution is needed when generalizing findings. (Note: Data from Hong Kong (S.A.R. of China) is available in the first wave of data collection. Data from mainland China were not included in the first data release due to fieldwork delays. The first wave of fieldwork in mainland China was completed in February 2024, and a second wave is expected to be completed in November-December 2024. All wave 1 and 2 data from mainland China will be part of the second dataset release in March 2025.)
Third, caution is also needed in interpreting cross-national differences as these may be influenced by matters of translation, different modes of assessment, cultural norms, interpretation of items and also of response scales, and seasonal effects arising from data being collected in different countries at different times of the year. Additionally, it’s important to acknowledge the cross-cultural differences in respondents’ interpretations and responses to the GFS survey items21,27. Although extensive pretesting and cognitive interviewing took place in each of the 22 countries included in the GFS, the modifications and revisions to the survey may not have accounted for all possible differences between countries when aligning the survey28.
Finally, the cross-sectional nature of the data is purely descriptive, and the sociodemographic associations do not account for covariates or interrelations between sociodemographic indicators. For example, although religious participation was associated with stronger belief in an afterlife, it remains unclear whether gender plays a role, as women were more likely to attend religious services and also exhibited higher levels of religious beliefs. There may also be variables that moderate this association, such as attachment to God. A key objective for future studies would be to investigate the underlying causes of variation in afterlife belief across countries, such as national differences in sex, age and educational level. Upcoming waves of the GFS will provide opportunities to explore these possibilities further.
Methods
The description of the methods below have been adapted from a team publication29. Further methodological detail is available elsewhere28,30,31,32,33,35.
Data
The Global Flourishing Study (GFS) is a study of 202,898 participants from 22 geographically and culturally diverse countries, with nationally representative sampling within each country, concerning the distribution of determinants of well-being. Wave 1 of the data included the following countries and territories: Argentina, Australia, Brazil, Egypt, Germany, Hong Kong (S.A.R of China, with mainland China also included from 2024 onwards), India, Indonesia, Israel, Japan, Kenya, Mexico, Nigeria, the Philippines, Poland, South Africa, Spain, Sweden, Tanzania, Turkey, United Kingdom, and the United States. The countries were selected to (a) maximize coverage of the world’s population, (b) ensure geographic, cultural, and religious diversity, and (c) prioritize feasibility and existing data collection infrastructure. Data collection was carried out by Gallup Inc. Data for Wave 1 were collected principally during 2023, with some countries beginning data collection in 2022 and exact dates varying by country28. Plans are in place to collect four additional waves of annual panel data on the participants from 2024 to 2027. The precise sampling design to ensure nationally representative samples varied by country and further details are available28. Survey items included aspects of well-being such as happiness, health, meaning, character, relationships, and financial stability36, along with other demographic, social, economic, political, religious, personality, childhood, community, health, and well-being variables. The data are publicly available through the Center for Open Science (https://www.cos.io/gfs). During the translation process, Gallup adhered to TRAPD model (translation, review, adjudication, pretesting, and documentation) for cross-cultural survey research (ccsg.isr.umich.edu/chapters/translation/overview).
Measures
Demographics Variables: Continuous age was classified as 18–24, 25–29, 30–39, 40–49, 50–59, 60–69, 70–79, and 80 or older. Gender was assessed as male, female, or other. Marital status was assessed as single/never married, married, separated, divorced, widowed, and domestic partner. Employment was assessed as employed, self-employed, retired, student, homemaker, unemployed and searching, and other. Education was assessed as up to 8 years, 9–15 years, and 16 + years. Service attendance was assessed as more than once/week, once/week, one-to-three times/month, a few times/year, or never. Immigration status was dichotomously assessed with: “Were you born in this country, or not?” Religious tradition/affiliation with categories of Christianity, Islam, Hinduism, Buddhism, Judaism, Sikhism, Baha’i, Jainism, Shinto, Taoism, Confucianism, Primal/Animist/Folk religion, Spiritism, African-Derived, some other religion, or no religion/atheist/agnostic; precise response categories varied by country (Johnson et al., 2023). Racial/ethnic identity was assessed in some, but not all, countries, with response categories varying by country. For additional details on the assessments see the COS GFS codebook30,31.
Outcome Variable: The afterlife belief measure comes from the following question: “I believe in life after death.” Respondents could answer Yes, No, or Unsure. This variable was dichotomized as Yes (1) vs. No/Unsure (0). As a post-hoc sensitivity analysis we will also report country means in the Online Supplement with the alternative dichotomization Yes/Unsure (1) vs. No (0), and furthermore report the absolute proportions in each country of “Unsure” responses.
Analysis
Descriptive statistics for the full sample, weighted to be nationally representative within each country, were estimated for each of the demographic variables. Nationally representative proportions for afterlife belief were estimated separately for each country and ordered from highest to lowest along, with 95% confidence intervals and standard deviations for each. Variation in proportions in afterlife belief across demographic categories were estimated, with all analyses initially conducted by country. Primary results consisted of a random effects meta-analyses of country-specific proportion of afterlife belief in each specific demographic category37,38 along with 95% confidence intervals, standard errors, lower and upper limits of a prediction interval across countries, heterogeneity (τ), and I2 for evidence concerning variation within a particular demographic variable across countries39. All meta-analyses were conducted in R40 using the metafor package41. Within each country, a global test of variation of outcome across levels of each particular demographic variable was conducted, and a pooled p-value42 across countries reported concerning evidence for variation within any country. Bonferroni corrected p-value thresholds are provided based on the seven sociodemographic variables (i.e., age, gender, marital status, employment status, education status, frequency of religious service attendance, and immigration status) that were included in the meta-analyses: p = .05/7 = 0.00743. Religious affiliation/tradition and race/ethnicity were used, when available, as control variables within country, but were not included in the meta-analyses since the availability of these response categories varied by country. As a supplementary analysis, population weighted meta-analyses were also conducted. All analyses were pre-registered with COS prior to data access (https://osf.io/6qyzh); all code to reproduce analyses are openly available in an online repository.
Missing data
Missing data on all variables was imputed using multivariate imputation by chained equations, and five imputed datasets were used44,45. To account for variation in the assessment of certain variables across countries (e.g., religious affiliation/tradition and race/ethnicity), the imputation process was conducted separately in each country. This within-country imputation approach ensured that the imputation models accurately reflected country-specific contexts and assessment methods. Sampling weights were included in the imputation models to account for specific-variable missingness that may have been related to probability of inclusion in the study.
Accounting for complex sampling design
The GFS used different sampling scheme across countries based on availability of existing panels and recruitment needs28. All analyses accounted for the complex survey design components by including weights, primary sampling units, and strata. Additional methodological detail, including accounting for the complex sampling design, is provided elsewhere32.
Data availability
The data that support the findings of this article are openly available on the Open Science Framework35. The specific dataset used was: Wave 1 non-sensitive Global data https://osf.io/sm4cd/ available February 2024 - March 2026 via preregistration and publicly from then onwards. The research questions, variables, and analytic plan for the current study were preregistered with the Center for Open Science prior to accessing data (https://osf.io/6qyzh). All code to reproduce analyses are openly available in an online repository33.
References
Sneath, E. H. (ed) Religion and the future life: The development of the belief in life after death (FH Revell Company, 1922).
Moreman, C. M. Beyond the threshold: Afterlife beliefs and experiences in world religions (Rowman & Littlefield, 2017).
Smith, H. The world’s religions: Our great wisdom traditions (HarperCollins, 1986).
Moreira-Almeida, A., Costa, M. A. & Coelho, H. S. Science of life after death (Springer, 2022).
Shariff, A. F. & Norenzayan, A. God is watching you: Priming God concepts increases prosocial behavior in an anonymous economic game. Psychol. Sci. 18 (9), 803–809 (2007).
Juergensmeyer, M. Terror in the mind of God: The global rise of religious violence (University of California Press, 2017).
Sosis, R. & Alcorta, C. S. Militants and Martyrs. In (eds Sagarin, R. D. & Taylor, T.) Natural security: A Darwinian approach to a dangerous world (105–124). University of California Press. (2008).
Bradshaw, M. & Ellison, C. G. Financial hardship and psychological distress: Exploring the buffering effects of religion. Soc. Sci. Med. 71 (1), 196–204 (2010).
Ellison, C. G., Burdette, A. M. & Hill, T. D. Blessed assurance: Religion, anxiety, and tranquility among US adults. Soc. Sci. Res. 38 (3), 656–667 (2009).
Flannelly, K. J., Ellison, C. G., Galek, K. & Silton, N. R. Belief in life-after-death, beliefs about the world, and psychiatric symptoms. J. Relig. Health. 51 (3), 651–662. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10943-012-9608-7 (2012).
Gallup More prone to believe in God than identify as religious. More likely to believe in heaven than in hell. gallup-international.com/survey-results-and-news/survey-result/more-prone-to-believe-in-god-than-identify-as-religious-more-likely-to-believe-in-heaven-than-in-hell (2023).
Trzebiatowska, M. & Bruce, S. Why are women more religious than men? (Oxford University Press, 2012).
Tornstam, L. Gerotranscendence-a theory about maturing into old age. J. Aging Identity. 1, 37–50 (1996).
Chen, Z. J., Cowden, R. G. & Streib, H. More spiritual than religious: Concurrent and longitudinal relations with personality traits, mystical experiences, and other individual characteristics. Front. Psychol. 13, 1025938 (2023).
Wixwat, M. & Saucier, G. Being spiritual but not religious. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 40, 121–125 (2021).
UK in the World Values Survey. God, heaven and hell, and life after death: data reveals UK’s low religious belief compared with other nations. , May 19 uk-values.org/news-comment/god-heaven-and-hell-and-life-after-death-data-reveals-uks-low-religious-belief-compared-with-other-nations (2023).
Hill, J. P. Faith and understanding: Specifying the impact of higher education on religious belief. J. Sci. Study Relig. 50 (3), 533–551 (2011).
Barrett, J. L. & Lanman, J. A. The science of religious beliefs. Religion 38 (2), 109–124 (2008).
Villanueva, R., Chen, Z. J. & Huang, Y. Analytic thinking, religiosity, and defensiveness against secularism: Absence of causality. Psychol. Relig. Spiritual. https://doi.org/10.1037/rel0000488 (2023).
Case, B. et al. Beyond a single story: The heterogeneity of human flourishing in 22 countries. Int. J. Wellbeing. 13 (4). https://doi.org/10.5502/ijw.v13i4.3555 (2023).
Cowden, R. G., Skinstad, D., Lomas, T., Johnson, B. R. & VanderWeele, T. J. Measuring wellbeing in the Global Flourishing Study: Insights from a cross-national analysis of cognitive interviews from 22 countries. Qual. Quant. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11135-024-01947-1 (2024).
Takahashi, M. Religion and Spirituality in Japan (ELM Grove Publishing, 2020).
Newman, D. B. & Graham, J. Religion and well-being. In (eds Diener, E., Oishi, S. & Tay, L.) Handbook of well-being (31–42). DEF. (2018).
Pew Research Center. Views on the afterlife. Majorities of U.S. adults say they believe in heaven, hell., pewresearch.org/religion/2021/11/23/views-on-the-afterlife(2021).
Butler, R. N. & Lewis, M. I. Aging and mental health: Positive psychosocial approaches (2nd ed.). Mosby. (1977).
Bell, A. Age period cohort analysis: a review of what we should and shouldn’t do. Ann. Hum. Biol. 47 (2), 208–217 (2020).
Johnson, K. A., Moon, J. W., VanderWeele, T. J., Schnitker, S. & Johnson, B. R. Assessing religion and spirituality in a cross-cultural sample: Development of religion and spirituality items for the Global Flourishing Study. Relig. Brain Behav. https://doi.org/10.1080/2153599X.2023.2217245 (2023).
Ritter, Z. et al. Global Flourishing Study Methodology (Gallup Inc, 2024).
VanderWeele, T. J. et al. (2025). The Global Flourishing Study and initial results. Nature Mental Health
Crabtree, S., English, C., Johnson, B. R., Ritter, Z. & VanderWeele, T. J. Global Flourishing Study: Questionnaire Development Report (Gallup Inc., 2021).
Crabtree, S., English, C., Johnson, B. R., Ritter, Z. & VanderWeele, T. J. Global Flourishing Study: 2024 Questionnaire Development Report (Gallup Inc, 2024). https://osf.io/y3t6m
Padgett, R. N., Bradshaw, M., Chen, Y., Jang, S. J., Shiba, K., Johnson, B. R., & VanderWeele, T. J. (2024a). Analytic methodology for demographic variation analyses for Wave 1 of the Global Flourishing Study. BMC Global & Public Health, in press.
Padgett, R. N., Bradshaw, M., Chen, Y., Jang, S. J., Shiba, K., Johnson, B. R., & VanderWeele, T. J. (2024b). Analytic methodology for childhood predictor analyses for Wave 1 of the Global Flourishing Study. BMC Global & Public Health, in press.
Padgett, R.N., Cowden, R.G., Chattopadhyay, M., Han, Y., Honohan, J., Ritter, Z., Srinivasan, R., Johnson, B.R., and VanderWeele, T.J. (2024c). Survey Sampling Design in Wave 1 of the Global Flourishing Study. European Journal of Epidemiology, in press.
Johnson, B. R. et al. The Global Flourishing Study. (2024). https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/3JTZ8
VanderWeele, T. J. On the promotion of human flourishing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(31), 8148–8156. (2017).
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T. & Rothstein, H. R. A basic introduction to fixed-effect and random-effects models for meta-analysis. Res. Synthesis Methods. 1 (2), 97–111. https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.12 (2010).
Hunter, J. E. & Schmidt, F. L. Fixed effects vs. random effects meta-analysis models: Implications for cumulative research knowledge. Int. J. Selection Assess. 8 (4), 275–292. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-2389.00156 (2000).
Mathur, M. B. & VanderWeele, T. J. Robust metrics and sensitivity analyses for meta-analyses of heterogeneous effects. Epidemiology 31 (3), 356–358 (2020).
R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. (2024). https://www.R-project.org/
Viechtbauer, W. Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J. Stat. Softw. 36 (3), 1–48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v036.i03 (2010).
Wilson, D. J. The harmonic mean p-value for combining dependent tests. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 116 (4), 1195–1200 (2019).
VanderWeele, T. J. & Mathur, M. B. Some desirable properties of the Bonferroni correction: Is the Bonferroni correction really so bad? Am. J. Epidemiol. 188 (3), 617–618 (2019).
Rubin, D. B. Multiple imputation after 18 + years. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 91 (434), 473–489 (1996).
van Burren, S. (2023). Flexible imputation of missing data (second edition). [Retrieved from https://stefvanburren.name/fimd/].
Acknowledgements
The Global Flourishing Study was supported by funding from the John Templeton Foundation (grant #61665), Templeton Religion Trust (#1308), Templeton World Charity Foundation (#0605), Well-Being for Planet Earth Foundation, Fetzer Institute (#4354), Well Being Trust, Paul L. Foster Family Foundation, and the David and Carol Myers Foundation. The opinions expressed in this publication are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of these organizations.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Z.J.C. conducted the analyses and wrote the manuscript. R.N.P. provided the code for data analysis. B.R.J. and T.J.V. acquired funding, coordinated data collection, participated in survey design, and supervised the development of the analytic code. R.G.C., A.M-A., T.B., B.V.K., R.N.P., B.R.J., and T.J.V. contributed to the review and editing of the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
Tyler VanderWeele reports consulting fees from Gloo Inc., along with shared revenue received by Harvard University in its license agreement with Gloo according to the University IP policy.
Additional information
Publisher’s note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Electronic Supplementary Material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Chen, Z.J., Cowden, R.G., Moreira-Almeida, A. et al. Sociodemographic variations of belief in life after death across 22 Countries. Sci Rep 15, 14310 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-83541-x
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-83541-x