Abstract
Many children who experience childhood adversity, whether in the form of threat or deprivation, develop adaptive competencies that lead to resilient functioning. Still, research has not succeeded in accurately predicting the level of resilient functioning by any kind of biomarkers, likely because it has sidelined the flexibility inherent in a construct that is situationally and developmentally variable. Whilst recent research acknowledges the importance of redefining resilience in order to reflect its dynamic nature after adversity, evidence for specific behaviors that are developmentally adaptive and dynamic throughout the lifespan is limited. We here propose a model in which resilient functioning is crucially dependent on the individual’s capability to flexibly synchronize with and segregate from another’s cognitive-affective, behavioral, and physiological states, known as ‘biobehavioral synchrony’. Such an adaptive interpersonal skill is rooted in (a) the early caregiving experience and its regulatory effects on an individual’s physiological stress reactivity, as well as (b) the development of self–other distinction which can be affected by childhood maltreatment. Bridging the gap between accounts of flexible resilient functioning and the latest thinking in biobehavioral synchrony, we will review behavioral and neurobiological evidence that threat and deprivation in childhood interfere with the development of dynamic, context-sensitive boundaries between self and other, mediated by the (right) tempo-parietal junction (a central neural hub for interpersonal synchronization), which puts the individual at risk for affective fusion or cut-off from others’ arousal states. Our proposed model charts a path for investigating the differential effects of maltreatment experiences and mechanisms for intergenerational transmission of non-sensitive caregiving. We conclude with metrics, data analysis methods, and strategies to facilitate flexible biobehavioral synchrony.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
The resilience paradox
Childhood maltreatment, defined as deliberate harm or the failure to prevent harm to individuals under 18 years, is a pervasive risk to development and is associated with psychopathology across the lifespan, affecting up to one in three minors (over 300 million children worldwide [1]). Nonetheless, most children who experience adversity exhibit adaptive functioning that surpasses expectations (resilient functioning, RF [2,3,4,5]). Indeed, studies show that the most common response to childhood adversity is predominantly adaptive or competent functioning [5,6,7], with an estimated two-thirds of individuals experiencing a potentially traumatic event following a resilient trajectory [3].
However, despite advances in our understanding of factors that promote RF after adversity, research has paradoxically not succeeded in accurately predicting positive outcomes based on its likely correlates [7,8,9]. Reasons for this include: i. the equifinal nature of RF in which many different factors can contribute to positive outcomes (e.g., biological and ecological), ii. oversimplification in which standard resilience assessments focus only on a small number of predictors (with small effect sizes), and iii. overlooking the importance of situational variability and developmental timing for RF [7,8,9,10]. Considering context-dependence in the study of RF prevents the ‘fallacy of uniform efficacy’ by allowing a specific resilience-promoting behavior to be efficient in one situation and not another, a cost-benefit trade-off that is at the core of almost any resilience promoting trait or behavior [11, 12]. The field of developmental psychology has responded to this criticism by redefining resilience as a skill, a behavioral and neural response that is flexible and adaptable across various situations and developmental stages, moving away from static and binary definitions based on isolated measurements of psychopathology in time [10, 13, 14]. To date, however, mechanistic accounts of the exact biobehavioral mechanisms associated with the dynamic nature of RF are limited.
In this review, we will first provide an overview of the different conceptualizations of RF and highlight the opportunities provided by a definition of RF that is inherently dynamic, i.e., sensitive to situational and developmental variability. We then bridge the gap from theoretical accounts of flexible RF to concrete biobehavioral mechanisms, by proposing that the extent to which an individual is able to flexibly align (synchronize) and retract (segregate) themselves from another person’s cognitive-affective, behavioral, and physiological state (biobehavioral synchrony), is one such flexible skill that confers RF after childhood adversity. In our model, we suggest that flexible biobehavioral synchrony as an index of RF is crucially dependent (a) on the early caregiving experience and both its regulatory effects on an individual’s physiological stress reactivity and interpersonal skills, as well as (b) the development of self–other distinction (SOD), which can be affected by childhood maltreatment. Unifying dynamic resilience accounts and the latest thinking in biobehavioral synchrony, we will review behavioral and neurobiological evidence that threat and deprivation in childhood interfere with the development of dynamic, context-sensitive boundaries between self and other, mediated by the tempo-parietal junction, putting the individual at risk for affective fusion or cut-off from others’ affective states and states of arousal. Finally, we present some hypotheses derived from this model about different RF trajectories after childhood adversity and intergenerational transmission of inflexible biobehavioural synchrony, concluding with suggestions for measurement, analyses, and prevention.
RF depends both on context and developmental stage
There is still an ongoing debate on how resilience could be best conceptualized or measured. RF generally refers to an individual who shows healthy and adaptive positive functioning over time in the aftermath of adversity [15,16,17]. Traditionally, resilience definitions encompassed the presence or absence of psychopathology in childhood [18,19,20] to more complex indices of socio-emotional functioning or individual traits and coping skills in adolescence and adulthood [21, 22]. We here, define RF as ‘the capacity of a dynamic system to adapt successfully to disturbances that threaten the viability, the function, or the development of that system’ [18, 23]. RF is thus dependent on both, the individual (e.g., neural, hormonal, and genetic make-up) and their environment (social, institutional, and cultural), as well as their interaction (see Ungar [19] for a constructionist view on resilience; see Russo and colleagues [20] for a review of neural, molecular and hormonal RF research and Elbau and colleagues [24] for an overview of genetic RF factors). According to a static conceptualization of RF [25,26,27,28,29], resilience measures often assess basic binary indicators such as academic failure or incarceration [30] or single- and multi-___domain composite scores assessing functioning across social and emotional domains [25] (e.g., reaching developmental language milestones [4], but see e.g., Feldman [27] for critique). Whilst static conceptualizations of resilience facilitate measurement, they lack ecological validity and negate both developmental timing and situational variability within which RF occurs [9, 10, 14, 20, 31].
Situational and developmental variability of RF is especially relevant for children who have experienced maltreatment. For example, studies documented more pronounced neurocognitive and behavioral changes depending on maltreatment type, onset, severity, and duration [32,33,34,35,36,37]. Therefore, RF likely presents differently along the spectrum of adverse experiences, depending on the ‘pressure’ or allostatic load the system works against [9] (e.g., a child with high maltreatment severity could be considered resilient at a lower threshold of functioning than a child with limited and mild adverse experiences, see [10]). RF is an inherently developmental process that partly unfolds as a function of caregiving from birth [27] and can occur at any stage in life [38], making developmental timing a key variable, too. A child may display RF in one developmental period and ___domain but not another [7, 16, 39]. Yoon and colleagues [7] strongly support a developmental conceptualization of resilience and have shown that resilience factors vary significantly throughout development, with unique individual, relational, and community protective elements emerging at different stages of life. For example, whilst resilience is primarily linked to caregiver warmth, emotional support, and cognitive stimulation in infancy [4, 40], caregiver well-being, effective parental engagement, as well as individual child characteristics and behaviors, such as child prosocial behavior become crucial for RF after childhood maltreatment in school-aged children [4, 41,42,43]. Adolescent RF post-adversity still hinges on strong caregiver support [21, 44, 45] including paternal acceptance [46], however, more peer-related activities and school engagement (e.g., involvement in extracurriculars, notably sports), emerge alongside as pivotal factors [21, 44, 45] together with positive attitudes and self-esteem [44, 46, 47]. In adults with childhood maltreatment experiences, RF is closely associated with a sense of community and belonging, encompassing religion, family, and friends [48,49,50,51]. Interpersonal strength, positive reframing, acceptance, and optimism are further resilience factors for adults who experienced childhood maltreatment [52,53,54]. Notably, a supportive family environment remains a strong influencing factor throughout the lifespan [48]. Thus what constitutes RF will look different at each stage of development as the interaction between children and their environment changes. This is why any theory of RF must accommodate the flexibility inherent in dynamic environments.
Resilience in the social context
The importance of social factors for RF has emerged across many studies [55, 56], confirming the significance of social support for the psychosocial development of young adults, who have experienced childhood maltreatment (e.g., [57]). Often, the combination of individual factors (such as gender) and social factors (e.g., maternal and sibling warmth, lower neighborhood crime victimization, greater social cohesion) best predicted whether a victimized individual experienced positive or negative psychosocial and economic outcomes during the transition to adulthood. This highlights the importance of a multidimensional conceptualization [58]. However, the empirical evidence base for mechanistic accounts of how social factors confer resilience after childhood maltreatment is limited and complicated by the use of non-comparable measures of resilience factors and a unidimensional conceptualization of social functioning, which treats structural (i.e., quantitative measures of social integration), functional (i.e., perceived social support), and qualitative indicators (i.e., positive and negative aspects of social relationships) in isolation [56]. Specifically, how close relationships with peers can buffer against the detrimental effects of early adverse childhood experiences remains mechanistically unclear. How can the adaptive advantages conferred by social-affiliative bonds be understood on a neurobiological level? Biobehavioral synchrony emerges as one potential mechanism underlying the resilience-promoting effects of social connections.
Biobehavioral synchrony refers to the temporal coordination of behavioral and biological responses between two or more interacting individuals [55] and likely embodies the adaptive skills that allow humans to perceive another person’s inner arousal state [59]. It may also foster the sharing and regulation of emotions, intensify social affiliation, empathy, and prosocial commitment [60], as well as promote learning [61], and facilitate adjustment to collective actions and group norms [62, 63]. This evolutionary mechanism captures the dynamic and reciprocal interplay between an individual’s physiological processes (e.g., heart rate), hormone release, and brain activity alongside observable behaviors like gaze or vocalizations. For instance, during a positive mother-infant interaction, the dyad’s heart rates and behaviors (e.g., mutual gaze or touch) might temporarily coordinate [61]. Biobehavioral synchrony plays a crucial role in all human relationships, in parental, romantic, friendship, and stranger interactions, and ‘being in sync’ with others has been correlated with improved stress management [64] and superior immune functioning [65], thereby offering a mechanism through which coordinated social actions alleviate stress and bolster resilience.
Based on this evidence, we have formulated a model of RF in which biobehavioral synchrony plays a crucial role in its development as a dynamic and developmentally-adaptive process. We will briefly summarize the different levels on which biobehavioral synchrony can be measured and what we know about biobehavioral synchrony dynamics throughout the lifespan. Biobehavioral synchrony occurs at multiple levels: b Behavioral synchrony, refers to the temporal coordination of observable actions between individuals, such as body movements, eye contact, speech, attention, touch and affect [66,67,68]. Behavioral synchrony is the most extensively investigated synchrony modality [69] with a major grounding in developmental research, where it is seen as a building block to the formation of attachment [70,71,72,73]. Physiological synchrony, mainly refers to the temporal coupling of functions of the autonomic nervous system (e.g., cardiac or electrodermal activity) and can be observed in established or stranger dyads [74,75,76]. It includes the temporal inter-relatedness of hormonal responses between interacting partners in close relationships: for example highly sensitive (but not less sensitive) mothers’ and their toddlers show synchronized cortisol reactivity in response to a laboratory-based challenge [77, 78]. Unsurprisingly, preliminary evidence exists for synchronized oxytocin levels during early parent-infant bonding [79, 80]. Physiological synchrony also varies by context, for example, stressful experimental conditions (e.g., difficult puzzles under time pressure) lead to disrupted parent-child respiratory sinus arrhythmia synchrony (RSA; indexing parasympathetic arousal) compared to less-structured, free-play tasks [81]. Biobehavioral synchrony is also dependent on the caregiver’s and child’s psychopathological state. For example, in school-aged children, mother–child dyads with a history of maternal depression showed lower RSA synchrony during a negative discussion task (e.g., discussing an issue on which the dyad regularly disagrees), but not during a pleasant, vacation-planning task [80, 82]. In a naturalistic study, less anxious mothers demonstrated greater physiological synchrony with their children throughout the day compared to anxious mothers. They only responded to ‘peak’ moments in their child’s arousal and were better at exhibiting ‘stress buffering’ behavior, effectively downregulating arousal when the overall arousal level of the dyad was high [83]. Interestingly, a recent meta-analysis showed that synchrony in various physiological systems is related to different outcomes [74]. Whereas overall measures of sympathetic nervous system synchrony were positively associated with favorable relationship outcomes, synchrony in the parasympathetic nervous system was negatively associated with relationship outcomes, indicating that overall high biobehavioral synchrony across different levels should not always be considered as the ‘optimal state’. Importantly, biobehavioral synchrony must be considered as a multimodal and flexible mechanism to adapt to changing environmental conditions. Neural synchrony, involves the coupling of neural activity between individuals [84, 85] and is driven by advances in brain imaging technology and a call for a more ecologically valid examination of social interactions [86,87,88,89]. Hyperscanning or the simultanoneous assessment of at least two interacting subjects [85, 90] enables scientists to map neural synchrony [85]. The proposed mechanisms of physiological and neural synchrony are associated with engagement and coupling of attentional states [91, 92], mutual prediction and understanding [93,94,95] emotional and cognitive alignment [96, 97], and co-regulation of arousal states [98, 99]. Gordon and colleagues [100] emphasize the importance of situational and developmental variability in shaping the tendency towards synchrony or segregation within a given social context in their model of flexible multimodal synchrony. Individual traits sensitize individuals to these situational variations. In this manner, the interplay of context and individual differences provides the foundation for adaptable and dynamic synchrony patterns across multiple situations and developmental states, encompassing behavioral, physiological, and neural aspects. In line with this, we suggest that a greater individual capacity to flexibly deploy interpersonal synchrony confers RF, that is tuning in (synchronizing) when it is adaptive and tuning out (segregating) when non-adaptive rather than constant states of alignment.
Biobehavioral synchrony in low- and high-risk dyads
The foundation of biobehavioral synchrony is laid in the last trimester of pregnancy and evolves constantly after birth, where a synchronization of physiological states serves to regulate the infant’s emotions [101, 102] and is achieved through the primary caregiver’s physical and behavioral attunement to the infant [103]. Positive early relational experiences with primary caregivers, enabled through parental sensitivity, competence, and warmth [4] become important facilitators for the development of the individual skills that are necessary for many RF’s, such as emotion and stress regulation, as well as interpersonal skills. It is the extent to which a primary caregiver and infant coordinate and align with one another mentally, behaviorally, and physiologically that confers RF through its regulatory effects on an individual’s physiological stress reactivity [72, 104]. Of note, these temporally coordinating events (behaviors, hormones, and peripheral-physiological and brain states) need not occur simultaneously in the interacting dyad but may also include temporally coordinated lagged events. The latter is sometimes referred to as contingency, for example when changes in partner A forward-predict changes in partner B (see [105]) and contrasted against an indirect concurrent (zero-lag) relationship, called synchrony, for example when both partners are in the same state at the same time. In this sense, both synchrony and contingency might impact RF across development. For example, when a caring interaction partner changes their behavior to calm a young person who faces a conditioned and potentially threatening stimulus (‘contingency’), this might support the child’s ability for extinction learning and thus lower the risk for developing post-traumatic stress symptoms. In a similar vein, if a neglected child’s state entrains to the positive emotional state of a caring interaction partner (‘synchrony’), this might help to up-regulate the child’s affect, broaden attention, and enhance flexibility [106]. Vice versa, a child adapting ‘contingently’ to a manipulative, abusive caregiver might be just as maladaptive for developing RF as being in concurrent synchrony with a highly impulsive-aggressive caregiver. In both latter cases, moving out of biobehavioural synchrony might bolster RF.
It has been suggested that synchrony has profound effects on social interactions that continue throughout the lifespan [106]. Diminished early synchrony experiences with the primary caregiver can alter neural, emotional, and behavioral development and have significant effects on the child’s stress and emotion regulation capacities and social competence [98, 107,108,109]. In line with this assumption, high-risk dyads (mothers suffering from depression and their infants) were shown to be less synchronous, i.e., had lower levels of gaze and touch synchrony, reduced coordination of affectionate touch with mutual gazing, and diminished maternal behavior [65, 72]. Furthermore, adversity has been associated with altered parent-child synchrony at multiple levels [107,108,109,110,111,112,113,114,115,116]. For example, elevated parenting stress has been linked to diminished behavioral synchrony between parents and children aged 3–14 [113]. Higher mother-infant cortisol synchrony was found in dyads from higher-SES compared to lower-SES backgrounds [111]. Mother-preschooler dyads with and without maltreatment experiences differed in within-dyad dynamic concordance in heart rate, such that variations in heart rate in one member of the dyad were associated with subsequent variations in the other only in those dyads without maltreatment experiences [112]. Hoyniak and colleagues [108] recently reported that adversity, across domains, was associated with decreased parent-child behavioral synchrony and that sociodemographic risk in particular was associated with decreased parent-child neural synchrony in the context of experimentally-induced stress. Collectively, these research findings demonstrate that adversity negatively impacts parent-child synchrony, potentially affecting social reciprocity.
On the behavioral level, childhood maltreatment has been associated with decreased cooperative behavior, more problematic peer relationships, and classroom behaviors such as higher levels of aggression or social withdrawal [117, 118]. On the other hand, maltreatment can be associated with indiscriminate friendliness [119, 120], sometimes also referred to as ‘costly altruism’ [121], which has recently been included as a diagnostic symptom of Disinhibited Social Engagement Disorder in the DSM-5. In line with this, Keil and colleagues [122] showed that maltreated children showed hyper-cooperativeness toward 9–16-year-old peers, which may serve to minimize hostility during initial encounters, but comes at the cost of potential exploitation by others. Given the strong association between cooperative behavior and biobehavioral synchrony [123, 124], the above findings of de- and increased cooperative behavior in young people with maltreatment experiences potentially indicate a diminished capacity for flexible synchronization (fusion, as indicated by hyper-cooperativeness) or segregation (cut-off, indicated by uncooperativeness) from others. This effect seems mediated by physiological arousal as previous research indicates that synchrony encourages greater cooperation mainly when paired with physiological arousal [123]. It is of interest then, that indices of heightened arousal such as decreased RSA [125] have been linked to maltreatment severity [126, 127] and physical abuse specifically whilst indices of decreased arousal (heightened RSA) were linked to maternal neglect history [128, 129]. These findings suggest differential effects of abuse and neglect on mother–child synchrony as is suggested by dimensional models of adversity [130, 131], as well as potential intergenerational transmission pathways of physiological stress management via biobehavioral synchrony [132, 133], in which RSA alterations in parents with their own histories of childhood maltreatment influence synchronous engagement with their offspring [107]. In line with this, parental childhood maltreatment correlates with changes in child interaction quality, possibly mediated by an altered oxytocin system (gauged by plasma levels [134]), and increased insecure attachment patterns, especially from parents who experienced emotional neglect [135]. This influence traverses several pathways, such as parental regulatory behaviors and physiological functions [126, 136], as well as interaction quality affecting attachment formation. Consequently, maltreated parents experiencing both altered levels of arousal and diminished emotion regulation might face challenges in co-regulating with their children in adolescence and adulthood [137].
SOD facilitates dynamic biobehavioral synchrony
Biobehavioral synchrony is mechanistically enabled through the blurring of the Self-Other distinction [SOD, 72, 138,139,140,141]. In the postnatal period, the infant relies heavily on the mother’s body, and a synchronous ‘oneness’ emerges between mother and child [142], but the synchrony and ‘oneness’ that characterizes the early years in normative development is inevitably subject to misalignment and moments of non-synchronous interactions, given that in typical social interactions, “as in any dynamical system, patterns of coordination form and dissipate” [143] (p. 233). In the real world, caregivers are more often unresponsive than responsive to their children (see for a review [144], see also: [62, 145, 146]. Consequently, dyads spend more time in mis‐coordinated states that are framed by precious moments of alignment, suggesting an “optimal midrange level” of behavioral synchrony and contingent interaction [147]. Psychoanalytic and developmental theories emphasize the importance of such match‐mismatch cycles for teaching infants how to tolerate moments of non‐attunement, how to repair the misunderstandings inherent in human dialog and because they provide vital opportunities to practice distinguishing self from other [142, 148,149,150]. The child increasingly learns to differentiate their own mental and arousal states from their caregiver through such non-synchronous interactions and their caregivers’ ability to both mirror the child’s emotional state (contingent mirroring [142, 148]), whilst clearly signaling that they are referring to their infants' mental state (marked mirroring [142, 148]). Gradually, achieving a separate sense of self in relationships with others whilst being able to experience closeness is one of the primary developmental tasks during adolescence [151, 152], as well as a marker of RF across the lifespan [153]. This important individuation can only be achieved if self–other distinction (defined as the capacity to experience a distinct and separate sense of self in relation to others [154]) has been enabled by a complex and dynamic interpersonal synchrony experience [100, 151] resulting in the capacity to respond flexibly to changing contexts throughout the lifespan.
The capacity to distinguish oneself from others emerges around 18–20 months and enables the distinction of one’s own body from someone else’s (perceptual level), the inhibition of automatic imitation tendencies (action level), representing others’ mental states that differ from our own (mental-state level), as well as our ability to empathize with others during incongruent emotional states [155] (see [156] for a detailed discussion of the development of SOD). On the neural level, the increasing ability to distinguish between self and others relies heavily on the right dorsal part of the temporoparietal junction (TPJ), as well as the medial prefrontal cortex [155, 157] (see [158]). Developmentally, TPJ undergoes significant structural and functional maturation at around 3–4 years, corresponding to significant improvements in perspective-taking at this age [159, 160]. Studies using near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) during mother–child dyadic interactions have shown that the TPJ is already specialized by 12–14 months of age to process social signals [161] and neural activity increases significantly in the TPJ in response to social stimuli across the first 24 months of life [162].
One common index of SOD is the assessment of the tendency to inhibit automatic imitation of others’ actions in experiments where individuals carry out motor responses while observing someone else’s congruent or incongruent (i.e., interfering) actions [156]. The degree to which observing incongruent trials interferes with one’s own action indexes how well SOD can be maintained. The right TPJ in particular has consistently been involved in upholding SOD, both mentally and behaviorally in imitation of action experiments [155]. Enabling SOD is hypothesized to be achieved through flexible switching between the representations of self and other (or enhance self when relevant / inhibit other, see [163]), enabled by the TPJ’s key role in attention reorientation (see e.g., [164, 165]). Interestingly, this overlaps with the central hub in the neural network associated with interpersonal synchronization as identified by Lotter and colleagues [166] in a recent meta-analysis, which included hyperscanning neuroimaging experiments. Across these studies, the most robust brain regional correlates of biobehavioral synchrony were in the right TPJ and left ventral prefrontal cortex. Extending this finding to a developmental population, Morgan and colleagues [167] showed that greater positive affective matching was related to greater synchrony in medial and lateral frontal and temporoparietal regions, confirming that biobehavioral synchrony involves sensory-integrative hubs with functional connections to mentalizing networks and thus overlaps with neural mechanisms of SOD (Fig. 1).
Figure 1 depicts the trajectories of flexible biobehavioural synchrony and the development of SOD as dynamic processes indexing RF over the lifespan. Strongly coupled synchrony in early infancy, mediated by a blurring of the boundary between the infant (self) and their primary caregiver (other), enabled by sensitive caregiving and co-regulation, which in turn fosters the development of both the infant's stress-coping and interpersonal skills. Beginning in childhood and culminating in later adolescence (when necessary individuation processes take place), positive synchrony alternates increasingly with moments of significant misalignment and uncoupling. These moments of rupture and repair provide the necessary context-dependent experience for the TPJ to master flexible switching between representations of self and other, ultimately establishing the basis for the ability to align (synchronize) and retract (segregate) from another person’s mental, and behavioral and physiological arousal state in adulthood in a dynamic and context-sensitive manner.
Maltreatment experiences impact the development of a flexible interpersonal synchrony system —a model
In childhood maltreatment, caregiving characterized by abuse and neglect significantly disrupts interpersonal synchrony [107,108,109, 111,112,113,114,115,116] and subsequent development of an ability to represent self and others efficiently [168], either because the caregiver was unable to accurately represent the emotional state of the infant (lack of congruency) or because they offered an excess of synchrony by overwhelming the infant with their own emotional states (lack of marking) [142, 148]. Assuming a cascading effect on domains of self–other representation we hypothesize that experiences of maltreatment impact on an individual’s capacity to flexibly deploy interpersonal synchrony according to situational and developmental demand, increasing the risk for emotional fusion or cut-off with others and subsequent relational difficulties and stress throughout an individual’s life. Such lack of flexible representation of self and others has already been identified as a driving force in different forms of psychopathologies, most comprehensively in borderline personality disorder [168]. Children who have experienced maltreatment, in the form of abuse and neglect, have been found to exhibit changes in SOD and its related domains, including perceptual, action, and mental SOD domains as discussed above. In terms of perceptual SOD for example, a recent study by Machorrinho and colleagues [169] showed that women who experienced intimate partner violence showed decreased levels of body ownership (the feeling of an individual’s body as their own) measured via the Rubber Hand Illusion [170] and higher levels of bodily dissociation, even after controlling for symptoms of PTSD and depression. Similarly, Talmon and Ginzburg [171] were able to demonstrate disrupted body boundaries in female students who experienced childhood maltreatment. In line with this, studies have also demonstrated that adults and children with PTSD and experiences of childhood maltreatment show preferences for larger interpersonal distances and aversion of social/affective touch, which is a key promoter of body ownership throughout development [172,173,174].
With regards to mental SOD, there is evidence that children with maltreatment experiences were delayed or less likely to pass false-belief tasks in experimental studies (ToM [175,176,177]) and showed an impoverished sense of self, accompanied by a diminished sense of identity [178]. A study by Burack and colleagues [179] has shown that maltreated children and adolescents were less successful in representing others perspectives and delayed in their social perspective-taking development compared to their non-maltreated peers, whilst other studies demonstrated deficits in emotion recognition and identification after childhood maltreatment (“affective ToM” [175]). Testing a specific aspect of SOD, a recent study by Hudson and colleagues [180] found that women with childhood experiences of abuse were less affected by incongruent imitation (inhibiting imitative behavior) suggesting they were less influenced by others’ perspectives due to changes in the representation of self and other. These differences in SOD capacity will have important consequences for social interactions throughout the lifespan [168], but even more so during adolescence when the ability to take someone else’s perspective is positively related to trusting others and social reciprocity [181]. Given the strong behavioral evidence for the impact of childhood maltreatment on the formation of perceptual, mental, and action SOD it is unsurprising that individuals with maltreatment experiences have shown altered neural activation patterns in key structures supporting biobehavioral synchrony and SOD, i.e., the TPJ, as well as the prefrontal cortex [182,183,184,185,186]. Cracco and colleagues [182] assessed rTPJ activation during spontaneous mentalizing in fMRI in 35 women with histories of childhood physical, sexual, and/or emotional abuse and 31 controls using a false belief task and found reduced activation in rTPJ in the abuse group for false vs true belief conditions and increased functional connectivity between rTPJ and dPFC, suggesting that altered mental SOD can last well into adulthood. Interestingly, these neural circuits involved in both attention and cognitive control were previously shown to be early post-trauma biomarkers of subsequent resilience [187]. Another study showed that childhood abuse was associated with increased resting state functional connectivity between the TPJ and the brainstem, which was driven by the number of types of abuse [183], suggesting a role for brainstem regions in biasing perspective-taking networks after trauma. Recent brain structural evidence also implicates the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, the medial prefrontal cortex, and the posterior superior temporal sulcus (including TPJ) in higher RF in adolescents [188]. The limited neuroimaging evidence that is predominantly available in adult populations thus suggests that the TPJ’s ability to switch in a flexible manner between the representations of self and others [163] is likely affected by childhood experiences of abuse and neglect. Future neuroimaging studies should extend the investigations of SOD domains to developmental populations (Fig. 2).
Figure 2 depicts the hypothesized effects of maltreatment on the development (infancy—childhood) and deployment (adulthood) of flexible biobehavioral synchrony (biobehavioral synchrony). We suggest the potential for threat and deprivation to exert differential effects on biobehavioral synchrony depending on whether (i.) violations of the self–other boundary have occurred that hindered the development of a clear distinction of self from other (e.g., sexual, emotional, and physical abuse), in turn increasing the risk for maladaptive affective merging (fusion) throughout the lifespan or whether (ii.) experiences of blurring of the SOD have been limited which can increase the risk for rigid, inflexible synchrony and potential affective cut-off. We suggest that rigid affective merging and cut-off are exaggerated responses on the continuum of synchrony and segregation.
A context-dependent and value-based model of resilience facilitation through flexible biobehavioral synchrony after maltreatment
In our hypothesized model we have proposed that RF after childhood maltreatment is conferred, at least in part, through flexible biobehavioral synchrony, i.e., the extent to which an individual is able to align with (synchronize), and retract (segregate) themselves from another person’s dynamic affective and arousal state depending on its changing adaptive value. We thus suggest that this flexible deployment of synchrony is guided by cost-benefit analyses which are central to almost any resilience-promoting traits or behaviors [11, 12]. In practice, tuning in and out of someone’s affective state could be beneficial in a scenario where one’s own mental state is characterized by negative affect and high arousal but the other’s by neutral or positive affect and low arousal (regulatory influence), whilst segregation would be adaptive when one’s own affective state is neutral or positive and the other’s affective state is negative with high arousal (agitatory influence). The level at which this capacity is developed throughout one’s life depends crucially on early caregiving experiences. These are influenced by the caregiver’s own history of biobehavioral synchrony, the child’s biological makeup, socio-ecological aspects of the caregiver-infant setting, and the adaptive capabilities of other social partners to align and synchronize flexibly. In our model, we propose that childhood adversity in the form of threat (e.g., sexual abuse, physical abuse, and exposure to domestic violence) and deprivation (e.g., emotional and physical neglect) interferes with the effective deployment of flexible synchrony via its main mechanism, SOD. Childhood adversity affects the development of the perceptual and mental SOD in ways that can diminish an individual’s ability to uphold the self–other boundary leading to a maladaptive merger (fusion) or rendering it rigid and inflexible (emotional cut-off) to the changing demands of dynamic social interaction, in essence pointing towards exaggerated synchrony and segregation behaviors. During adolescence, in which the brain undergoes significant neuroplastic changes (see [9]) and is particularly sensitive to social experiences outside the family, the ability to flexibly synch and segregate from others might contribute significantly to the quality of adolescents’ social experiences and relationships, thereby indirectly impacting RF (see [189]). It is during this developmental period that established SOD is transferred to new (e.g., romantic or peer) relationships, and flexible biobehavioral synchrony capacities might be challenged and reshaped across multiple social contexts. Here, it has to be taken into account that both, biological changes in the individual response system (such as changes in the HPA functioning, development of rTPJ and prefrontal cortex functions), as well as the expansion of the social network might impact and change the flexibility of biobehavioral synchrony during development. Thus, the model proposed here suggests that there cannot be a uniform response to experiences of adversity in childhood as the trajectories of RF will be dependent on both the maltreatment context and the developmental stage in which RF is assessed. However, our model provides a framework within which hypotheses regarding the role of flexible synchrony in RF can be tested. For example, based on the model we outlined here, one testable assumption relates to the effect of early caregiving experiences on the flexible deployment of synchrony based on its context/adaptive value. Individuals who have experienced childhood maltreatment might exhibit less flexible deployment of biobehavioral synchrony in a VR-simulated social contagion experiment as indexed by higher susceptibility to both, negatively and positively valenced emotional contagion as opposed to non-maltreated peers who would be expected to be more susceptible to positive contagion than negative in line with its adaptive value. More nuanced testable assumptions from the model pertain to the influence of maltreatment type on RF through the flexible application of biobehavioral synchrony. Future studies could investigate if a childhood characterized predominantly by neglect is associated with diminished flexibility of biobehavioral synchrony with others in the form of emotional cut-off (i.e., difficulty ‘tuning in’), due to the absence of early childhood experiences of ‘oneness’ and co-regulation. Vice versa, experiences of a breakdown in one’s physical and emotional boundaries as is the case in emotional, sexual, and physical abuse could be associated with a diminished flexibility of biobehavioral synchrony in the form of emotional fusion (i.e., difficulty ‘tuning out’ and distinguishing self from other) as has been suggested in the literature [180]. Importantly, the model allows for testable assumptions to be made about the neurophysiological correlates involved in RF facilitated through flexible biobehavioral synchrony with techniques outlined in more detail below (Fig. 3).
Figure 3 shows our proposed model in which RF is crucially dependent on the individual’s capability to flexibly synchronize with and segregate from another’s cognitive-affective, behavioral, and physiological states, known as ‘biobehavioral synchrony’. Such an adaptive interpersonal skill is rooted in i. the early caregiving experience and its regulatory effects on an individual’s physiological stress reactivity, as well as ii. the development of SOD which can be affected by childhood maltreatment. Flexible biobehavioral synchrony exerts its effects on RF in direct (i.e., synchrony profiles guided by cost-benefit analyses), as well as indirect ways (e.g., interplay with an individual’s neural function and social architecture). Assuming a cascading effect on domains of SOD we hypothesize that experiences of maltreatment impact an individual’s capacity to flexibly deploy interpersonal synchrony according to situational and developmental demand (synching or segregating), increasing the risk for emotional fusion or cut-off with others and subsequent relational difficulties and stress throughout an individual’s life.
Assessments
For a flexible and active conceptualization of RF we consider the following tools as potentially helpful for future studies (see also Table 1). Given the context- and age-sensitivity of RF, measures of RF should not only focus on the individual itself, but need to consider the environmental context, the significant social relationships, as well as the individual’s current life stage including the maturation of his/her cognitive and emotional skills. Advancing the study of RF in the light of biobehavioral synchrony thus requires developmental-sensitive assessments of the transactional interactions between person and environment. Several questionnaires for RF have been developed and also adapted to different age groups, including adolescents and whilst some are based on ecological models and include the connectedness and availability of a supportive environment (e.g., family or wider community), questionnaire assessment of RF as a truly interactive process is still scarce. Self-report measures that explicitly assess aspects of social interaction experiences, including aspects of SOD such as “identity fusion” in romantic or close relationships (e.g., the inclusion of other in the self scale) [190] might be helpful. Such assessments should be supplemented by developmentally appropriate experimental assessments of SOD based on the common coding theory, for example through experimental paradigms of inhibition of automatic imitation of others’ actions [156]. It is essential to confirm that tools are sensitive to interindividual differences and provide age-specific equivalents in order to validly and consistently capture identical cognitive-affective functions. This necessitates studying measurements tailored to specific ages for consistent concepts over periods, analyzing their associations with other indicators, and bolstering collaboration between labs to standardize testing techniques (see also [191]).
Considering RF in light of the flexible biobehavioral synchrony model also requires assessments of multimodal biobehavioral synchrony paradigms during exemplary interaction situations. For example, assessing RF in the social context of threat might be measurable as the ability to engage in, and synchronize with potentially non-threatening and caring interaction partners, while actively disengaging from interpersonal synchrony in response to an interaction partner sending signals of threat. Assessing RF in the social context of deprivation by contrast might be assessed by flexibility indices of multimodal biobehavioral synchrony in response to different cooperative and emotionally evoking situations.
With respect to data analytic challenges, this conceptualization requires novel and innovative tools for future empirical studies. The current conceptualization of RF calls for advanced statistical techniques to capture these complexities (see also [10] for statistical recommendations according to complexity theory). To account for developmental and context-dependent changes in RF, structural equation modeling which allows the integration of multiple levels of data over time, and Latent Growth Curve Modeling, which captures changes over time, as well as Network Analysis, which examines interrelationships among variables might be promising techniques.
In the past, most studies on biobehavioral synchrony focused on aggregated parameters of synchrony, i.e., the total or average amount or on phases during which the highest level of synchrony is reached, but the current conceptualization of resilience as a dynamic process of engaging in and disengaging from synchrony accordingly requires analyses of such dynamics. This requirement applies across timescales, ranging from biobehavioral synchrony measures in milliseconds to the entire lifespan. For example, methods that are capable of catching sequential biobehavioral synchrony processes, such as Granger causality or partial directed coherence, allow analyses of how one time series influences the time series of another person in multivariate data analyses. It might be particularly promising to analyze whether more resilient subjects more often are leaders or followers when moving into or out of synchrony during different social interaction contexts. Mayo and Gordon [192] recently suggested novel synchrony analyses following the idea of complex dynamical systems. At the core of this model is the concept of meta-stability as a marker of such a flexible interpersonal system. Furthermore, as there is growing evidence that the synchrony of body and mind is distinct and their relationship is dependent on context [193], another methodological challenge concerns the complete multimodal assessment and analysis of multimodal biobehavioral synchrony data. There is evidence that different measures of biobehavioral synchrony follow different time scales, for example, synchronized neural activity was found in a much faster timescale (milliseconds) than behavioral synchrony. It is thus conceivable that such processes might be dynamically linked but need to be analyzed within a multi-level design to reach a better understanding of this complex relationship and how it relates to RF. Finally, according to conceptualizations of resilience as a dynamic and active process, it is also important to assess this process from a life-span perspective.
Implications for prevention programs and targeted interventions
A better understanding of RF within the model of flexible biobehavioral synchrony might also be informative for personalizing programs to foster RF after childhood maltreatment. We suggest that targeting RF in the individual’s social context and addressing both, psychological and neurobiological aspects in interacting subjects simultaneously might lead to more effective prevention and intervention programs. As outlined above, we consider that RF benefits if the program moves beyond single subjects but includes significant others, spanning the whole social network or even community level. Here, we will briefly outline some preliminary findings, mainly derived from proof-of-concept studies, based on the model proposed here which might stimulate further clinical trials targeting resilience in the future.
The pivotal role of the rTPJ for biobehavioral synchrony and SOD suggests a potential avenue for neuro-based interventions targeting rTPJ to augment RF. Using transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), Santiesteban and colleagues [194] showed that excitatory (anodal) stimulation over the TPJ improved both, the control of imitative tendencies (i.e., distinguishing in favor of self-representations) and visual perspective-taking (i.e., distinguishing in favor of other representations). At the same time, it did not affect social judgments which did not lead to a conflict of self and other representations, thus highlighting the potential for tDCS to be used as a tool to aid SOD also in clinical populations. However, on a more critical note, the evidence for potential enhancement through tDCS is promising but mixed and many tDCS studies have reported no significant enhancing effects on other complex socio–cognitive abilities including theory of mind, empathy, emotion recognition, and joint attention [195,196,197,198]. Furthermore, Kohl et al. [199] showed that the rTPJ can also be specifically enhanced by fNIRS-based neurofeedback. Interestingly, while this study showed that reorienting of attention improved after four daily training sessions in healthy adult subjects, no specific effect was found for perspective-taking and SOD. Thus, enhancing TPJ functions does not automatically lead to improved SOD. A remaining question is whether or not the flexibility to push into and segregate from synchrony can be enhanced specifically, either by behavioral training or pharmacologically (e.g., via oxytocin stimulation), via dual brain stimulation (e.g., by simultaneous transcranial altering stimulation (tACS), or via synchrony-based physiological or neural neurofeedback.
On the behavioral level, several studies have shown that promising candidates for enhancing biobehavioral synchrony include (i) coordinating or synchronizing body movements [200], (ii) making music together [201] (see also [202], (iii) playing cooperative or interactive games (for a review [203], and (iv) engaging in shared eye contact or joint attention [204] (for a review see [205]). Future research should investigate if such interventions have the potential to stimulate patterns of biobehavioral synchrony which are characterized by rigidity and inflexibility or those characterized by hyper-synchronicity to more flexible and responsive synchrony patterns. This, in turn, could stimulate patterns of adaptive responding in dynamic social interactions in children who have experienced maltreatment.
On the pharmacological level, so far only a few studies demonstrated that maternal chemo-signals might increase biobehavioral synchrony between parents and infants and can also increase biobehavioral synchrony between an infant and an unfamiliar person [206]. Furthermore, the administration of intranasal oxytocin can effectively boost inter-brain synchrony [207] and promote behavioral synchrony in adult participants [208]. Another technique to manipulate biobehavioral synchrony in interacting individuals is to provide ANS-based feedback, for instance, based on heart rate variability. There is preliminary evidence that a dual feedback system may help to increase interpersonal synchrony in a therapeutic setting [209] or promote empathy and social entrainment between two people [210]. Besides heart rate variability, skin conductance has been used to provide (simulated) dyadic feedback [211]. Other studies have provided ANS-based biofeedback based on breathing rhythm [195, 196, 212]. As synchronizing and desynchronizing from arousal and Autonomic Nervous System (ANS) activity during close interactions might be particularly crucial for intergenerational transmission of maltreatment and for RF, such interventional methods are considered particularly promising for future trials.
Furthermore, brain stimulation can be used simultaneously in interacting subjects to directly modulate biobehavioral synchrony on the neural level. In a hyper-tACS protocol, the stimulation can be applied with the same or different frequency and phase. While same-phase–same-frequency stimulation is expected to enhance biobehavioral synchrony, different-phase-same-frequency or different-phase-different-frequency stimulation is expected to reduce biobehavioral synchrony. Modulation of inter-brain synchronization can either impair or improve joint action coordination [213] and thus might be particularly promising for supporting both, pulling into but also segregating from biobehavioral synchrony. Finally, preliminary evidence shows that simultaneous EEG-based neurofeedback can be applied to specifically modulate neural synchrony in interacting subjects. Fewer studies tried to employ neurofeedback based on simultaneous fMRI or fNIRS signals. Although these techniques are still in infancy they are promising candidate methods to personalize and manipulate the neural substrate of RF in interactive contexts.
Conclusions
In advancing our understanding and the promotion of RF following childhood adversity, it is imperative to delve profoundly into the interplay between ecological factors, particularly the micro-level social dynamic interactions, and individual neurobiological backgrounds. The complexity inherent in resilience research necessitates a bio-convergent approach, bridging the expertise of life sciences and data sciences. By fostering interdisciplinary collaboration, we can overcome the current barriers, enriching our understanding of biobehavioral synchrony, and its pivotal role in RF, especially in the face of early life adversity.
References
WHO. Child maltreatment. Word Health Organ. 2022. https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/child-maltreatment.
Kessler RC. Posttraumatic stress disorder in the national comorbidity survey. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;52:1048.
Galatzer-Levy IR, Huang SH, Bonanno GA. Trajectories of resilience and dysfunction following potential trauma: a review and statistical evaluation. Clin Psychol Rev. 2018;63:41–55.
Holmes MR, Yoon S, Berg KA, Cage JL, Perzynski AT. Promoting the development of resilient academic functioning in maltreated children. Child Abuse Negl. 2018;75:92–103.
Holmes M, Yoon S, Voith L, Kobulsky J, Steigerwald S. Resilience in physically abused children: protective factors for aggression. Behav Sci. 2015;5:176–89.
Kalisch R, Müller MB, Tüscher O. A conceptual framework for the neurobiological study of resilience. Behav Brain Sci. 2015;38:e92.
Yoon S, Howell K, Dillard R, Shockley McCarthy K, Rae Napier T, Pei F. Resilience following child maltreatment: definitional considerations and developmental variations. Trauma Violence Abuse. 2021;22:541–59.
Bonanno GA. The end of trauma: how the new science of resilience is changing how we think about PTSD. Basic Books. 2021. https://books.google.de/books?id=fAISEAAAQBAJ.
Malhi GS, Das P, Bell E, Mattingly G, Mannie Z. Modelling resilience in adolescence and adversity: a novel framework to inform research and practice. Transl Psychiatry. 2019;9:316.
Ioannidis K, Askelund AD, Kievit RA, Van Harmelen A-L. The complex neurobiology of resilient functioning after childhood maltreatment. BMC Med. 2020;18:32.
Bonanno GA, Burton CL. Regulatory flexibility: an individual differences perspective on coping and emotion regulation. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2013;8:591–612.
Kalokerinos EK, Greenaway KH, Casey JP. Context shapes social judgments of positive emotion suppression and expression. Emotion. 2017;17:169–86.
Leys C, Arnal C, Wollast R, Rolin H, Kotsou I, Fossion P. Perspectives on resilience: Personality trait or skill? Eur J Trauma Dissociation. 2020;4:100074.
Rutter M. Resilience as a dynamic concept. Dev Psychopathol. 2012;24:335–44.
Wu G, Feder A, Cohen H, Kim JJ, Calderon S, Charney DS, et al. Understanding resilience. Front Behav Neurosci. 2013. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00010.
Luthar SS, Cicchetti D. The construct of resilience: Implications for interventions and social policies. Dev Psychopathol. 2000;12:857–85.
Southwick SM, Bonanno GA, Masten AS, Panter-Brick C, Yehuda R. Resilience definitions, theory, and challenges: interdisciplinary perspectives. Eur J Psychotraumatology. 2014;5:25338.
Masten AS. Ordinary magic: resilience in development. New York: The Guilford Press; 2014.
Ungar M. A constructionist discourse on resilience: multiple contexts, multiple realities among at-risk children and youth. Youth Soc. 2004;35:341–65.
Russo SJ, Murrough JW, Han M-H, Charney DS, Nestler EJ. Neurobiology of resilience. Nat Neurosci. 2012;15:1475–84.
Williams J, Nelson-Gardell D. Predicting resilience in sexually abused adolescents. Child Abuse Negl. 2012;36:53–63.
Deblinger E, Pollio E, Runyon MK, Steer RA. Improvements in personal resiliency among youth who have completed trauma-focused cognitive behavioral therapy: a preliminary examination. Child Abuse Negl. 2017;65:132–9.
Masten AS. Global perspectives on resilience in children and youth. Child Dev. 2014;85:6–20.
Elbau IG, Cruceanu C, Binder EB. Genetics of resilience: gene-by-environment interaction studies as a tool to dissect mechanisms of resilience. Biol Psychiatry. 2019;86:433–42.
Connor KM, Davidson JRT. Development of a new resilience scale: the Connor-Davidson resilience scale (CD-RISC). Depress Anxiety. 2003;18:76–82.
Holz NE, Tost H, Meyer-Lindenberg A. Resilience and the brain: a key role for regulatory circuits linked to social stress and support. Mol Psychiatry. 2020;25:379–96.
Feldman R. What is resilience: an affiliative neuroscience approach. World Psychiatry. 2020;19:132–50.
Block AA. The Organized Crime Control Act, 1970: historical issues and public policy. Public Hist. 1980;2:39–59.
Southwick SM, Charney DS. The science of resilience: implications for the prevention and treatment of depression. Science. 2012;338:79–82.
Topitzes J, Mersky JP, Dezen KA, Reynolds AJ. Adult resilience among maltreated children: a prospective investigation of main effect and mediating models. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2013;35:937–49.
Kalisch R, Baker DG, Basten U, Boks MP, Bonanno GA, Brummelman E, et al. The resilience framework as a strategy to combat stress-related disorders. Nat Hum Behav. 2017;1:784–90.
Puetz VB, Viding E, Gerin MI, Pingault J-B, Sethi A, Knodt AR, et al. Investigating patterns of neural response associated with childhood abuse v. childhood neglect. Psychol Med. 2020;50:1398–407.
Puetz VB, Kohn N, Dahmen B, Zvyagintsev M, Schüppen A, Schultz RT, et al. Neural response to social rejection in children with early separation experiences. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2014;53:1328–.e8.
McCrory EJ, Puetz VB, Maguire EA, Mechelli A, Palmer A, Gerin MI, et al. Autobiographical memory: a candidate latent vulnerability mechanism for psychiatric disorder following childhood maltreatment. Br J Psychiatry. 2017;211:216–22.
Arnow BA. Relationships between childhood maltreatment, adult health and psychiatric outcomes, and medical utilization. J Clin Psychiatry. 2004;65:10–15.
Kaufman J. Depressive disorders in maltreated children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1991;30:257–65.
Puetz VB, Zweerings J, Dahmen B, Ruf C, Scharke W, Herpertz-Dahlmann B, et al. Multidimensional assessment of neuroendocrine and psychopathological profiles in maltreated youth. J Neural Transm. 2016;123:1095–106.
Werner EE. Vulnerability and resiliency: a longitudinal perspective. In: Brambring M, Lösel F, Skowronek H, editors. Children at risk: assessment, longitudinal research, and intervention. Berlin: de Gruyter; 1989, pp. 158–72.
Cowell RA, Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA, Toth SL. Childhood maltreatment and its effect on neurocognitive functioning: timing and chronicity matter. Dev Psychopathol. 2015;27:521–33.
Sattler KMP, Font SA. Resilience in young children involved with child protective services. Child Abuse Negl. 2018;75:104–14.
Bell JJ, Hardy L, Beattie S. ‘Enhancing mental toughness and performance under pressure in elite young cricketers: a 2-year longitudinal intervention’: correction to Bell et al. (2013). Sport Exerc Perform Psychol. 2013;2:297–297.
Holmes C, Levy M, Smith A, Pinne S, Neese P. A model for creating a supportive trauma-informed culture for children in preschool settings. J Child Fam Stud. 2015;24:1650–9.
Bell T, Romano E, Flynn RJ. Profiles and predictors of behavioral resilience among children in child welfare. Child Abuse Negl. 2015;48:92–103.
Asgeirsdottir BB, Gudjonsson GH, Sigurdsson JF, Sigfusdottir ID. Protective processes for depressed mood and anger among sexually abused adolescents: the importance of self-esteem. Personal Individ Differ. 2010;49:402–7.
Guibord M, Bell T, Romano E, Rouillard L. Risk and protective factors for depression and substance use in an adolescent child welfare sample. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2011;33:2127–37.
Davidson-Arad B, Navaro-Bitton I. Resilience among adolescents in foster care. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2015;59:63–70.
Maples LA, Park SS, Nolen JP, Rosén LA. Resilience to Childhood Abuse and Neglect in College Students. J Aggress Maltreatment Trauma. 2014;23:1001–19.
Bradley B, Davis TA, Wingo AP, Mercer KB, Ressler KJ. Family environment and adult resilience: contributions of positive parenting and the oxytocin receptor gene. Eur J Psychotraumatology. 2013;4:21659.
Greenfield EA, Marks NF. Identifying experiences of physical and psychological violence in childhood that jeopardize mental health in adulthood. Child Abuse Negl. 2010;34:161–71.
Banyard V, Hamby S, Grych J. Health effects of adverse childhood events: identifying promising protective factors at the intersection of mental and physical well-being. Child Abuse Negl. 2017;65:88–98.
Goldstein AL, Faulkner B, Wekerle C. The relationship among internal resilience, smoking, alcohol use, and depression symptoms in emerging adults transitioning out of child welfare. Child Abuse Negl. 2013;37:22–32.
Kapoor KK, Tamilmani K, Rana NP, Patil P, Dwivedi YK, Nerur S. Advances in social media research: past, present and future. Inf Syst Front. 2018;20:531–58.
Mohr D, Rosén LA. The impact of protective factors on posttraumatic growth for college student survivors of childhood maltreatment. J Aggress Maltreatment Trauma. 2017;26:756–71.
Lumley MN, McArthur BA. Protection from depression following emotional maltreatment: the unique role of positive schemas. Int J Cogn Ther. 2016;9:327–43.
Gartland D, Riggs E, Muyeen S, Giallo R, Afifi TO, MacMillan H, et al. What factors are associated with resilient outcomes in children exposed to social adversity? A systematic review. BMJ Open. 2019;9:e024870.
Latham RM, Quilter E, Arseneault L, Danese A, Moffitt TE, Newbury JB, et al. Childhood maltreatment and poor functional outcomes at the transition to adulthood: a comparison of prospective informant- and retrospective self-reports of maltreatment. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2021;56:1161–73.
Pepin EN, Banyard VL. Social support: a mediator between child maltreatment and developmental outcomes. J Youth Adolesc. 2006;35:612–25.
Latham RM, Meehan AJ, Arseneault L, Stahl D, Danese A, Fisher HL. Development of an individualized risk calculator for poor functioning in young people victimized during childhood: a longitudinal cohort study. Child Abuse Negl. 2019;98:104188.
Mizugaki S, Maehara Y, Okanoya K, Myowa-Yamakoshi M. The power of an infant’s smile: maternal physiological responses to infant emotional expressions. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0129672.
Mogan R, Fischer R, Bulbulia JA. To be in synchrony or not? A meta-analysis of synchrony’s effects on behavior, perception, cognition and affect. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2017;72:13–20.
Pan Y, Novembre G, Song B, Zhu Y, Hu Y. Dual brain stimulation enhances interpersonal learning through spontaneous movement synchrony. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2021;16:210–21.
Wiltermuth SS, Heath C. Synchrony and cooperation. Psychol Sci. 2009;20:1–5.
Reinero DA, Dikker S, Van Bavel JJ. Inter-brain synchrony in teams predicts collective performance. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2021;16:43–57.
Halevi G, Djalovski A, Kanat-Maymon Y, Yirmiya K, Zagoory-Sharon O, Koren L, et al. The social transmission of risk: maternal stress physiology, synchronous parenting, and well-being mediate the effects of war exposure on child psychopathology. J Abnorm Psychol. 2017;126:1087–103.
Ulmer-Yaniv A, Djalovski A, Priel A, Zagoory-Sharon O, Feldman R. Maternal depression alters stress and immune biomarkers in mother and child. Depress Anxiety. 2018;35:1145–57.
Gaziv G, Noy L, Liron Y, Alon U. A reduced-dimensionality approach to uncovering dyadic modes of body motion in conversations. PLoS One. 2017;12:e0170786.
Tschacher W, Rees GM, Ramseyer F. Nonverbal synchrony and affect in dyadic interactions. Front Psychol. 2014. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01323.
Gordon I, Gilboa A, Cohen S, Milstein N, Haimovich N, Pinhasi S, et al. Physiological and behavioral synchrony predict group cohesion and performance. Sci Rep. 2020;10:8484.
Sebanz N, Bekkering H, Knoblich G. Joint action: bodies and minds moving together. Trends Cogn Sci. 2006;10:70–76.
Feldman R, Zagoory-Sharon O, Weisman O, Schneiderman I, Gordon I, Maoz R, et al. Sensitive parenting is associated with plasma oxytocin and polymorphisms in the OXTR and CD38 genes. Oxytocin Soc Bonds Dev. 2012;72:175–81.
Carotenuto M, Esposito M, Parisi L, Gallai B, Marotta R, Di Dona A, et al. Attachment styles in children affected by migraine without aura. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2013;9:1513–9.
Feldman R. Parent-infant synchrony: a biobehavioral model of mutual influences in the formation of affiliative bonds: parent-infant synchrony. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 2012;77:42–51.
Harel H, Gordon I, Geva R, Feldman R. Gaze behaviors of preterm and full-term infants in nonsocial and social contexts of increasing dynamics: visual recognition, attention regulation, and gaze synchrony: GAZE BEHAVIOR IN PRETERMS. Infancy. 2011;16:69–90.
Mayo O, Lavidor M, Gordon I. Interpersonal autonomic nervous system synchrony and its association to relationship and performance—a systematic review and meta-analysis. Physiol Behav. 2021;235:113391.
Bar-Kalifa E, Prinz JN, Atzil-Slonim D, Rubel JA, Lutz W, Rafaeli E. Physiological synchrony and therapeutic alliance in an imagery-based treatment. J Couns Psychol. 2019;66:508–17.
Palumbo RV, Marraccini ME, Weyandt LL, Wilder-Smith O, McGee HA, Liu S, et al. Interpersonal autonomic physiology: a systematic review of the literature. Personal Soc Psychol Rev. 2017;21:99–141.
Sethre-Hofstad L. Attunement of maternal and child adrenocortical response to child challenge. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2002;27:731–47.
Van Bakel HJA, Riksen-Walraven JM. Adrenocortical and behavioral attunement in parents with 1-year-old infants. Dev Psychobiol. 2008;50:196–201.
Szymanska M, Schneider M, Chateau‐Smith C, Nezelof S, Vulliez‐Coady L. Psychophysiological effects of oxytocin on parent–child interactions: a literature review on oxytocin and parent–child interactions. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2017;71:690–705.
Scatliffe N, Casavant S, Vittner D, Cong X. Oxytocin and early parent-infant interactions: a systematic review. Int J Nurs Sci. 2019;6:445–53.
Lunkenheimer E, Tiberio SS, Skoranski AM, Buss KA, Cole PM. Parent‐child coregulation of parasympathetic processes varies by social context and risk for psychopathology. Psychophysiology. 2018;55:e12985.
Woody ML, Feurer C, Sosoo EE, Hastings PD, Gibb BE. Synchrony of physiological activity during mother–child interaction: moderation by maternal history of major depressive disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2016;57:843–50.
Smith CG, Jones EJH, Charman T, Clackson K, Mirza FU, Wass SV. Anxious parents show higher physiological synchrony with their infants. Psychol Med. 2022;52:3040–50.
Hasson U, Frith CD. Mirroring and beyond: coupled dynamics as a generalized framework for modelling social interactions. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2016;371:20150366.
Nam CS, Choo S, Huang J, Park J. Brain-to-brain neural synchrony during social interactions: a systematic review on hyperscanning studies. Appl Sci. 2020;10:6669.
Liu D, Liu S, Liu X, Zhang C, Li A, Jin C, et al. Interactive brain activity: review and progress on EEG-based hyperscanning in social interactions. Front Psychol. 2018;9:1862.
Montague PR, Berns GS, Cohen JD, McClure SM, Pagnoni G, Dhamala M, et al. Hyperscanning: simultaneous fMRI during linked social interactions. Neuroimage. 2002;16:1159–64.
Redcay E, Schilbach L. Using second-person neuroscience to elucidate the mechanisms of social interaction. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2019;20:495–505.
Shamay-Tsoory SG, Mendelsohn A. Real-life neuroscience: an ecological approach to brain and behavior research. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2019;14:841–59.
Reindl V, Gerloff C, Scharke W, Konrad K. Brain-to-brain synchrony in parent-child dyads and the relationship with emotion regulation revealed by fNIRS-based hyperscanning. Neuroimage. 2018;178:493–502.
Dikker S, Wan L, Davidesco I, Kaggen L, Oostrik M, McClintock J, et al. Brain-to-brain synchrony tracks real-world dynamic group interactions in the classroom. Curr Biol. 2017;27:1375–80.
Stuldreher IV, Thammasan N, Van Erp JBF, Brouwer A-M. Physiological synchrony in EEG, electrodermal activity and heart rate detects attentionally relevant events in time. Front Neurosci. 2020;14:575521.
Nguyen T, Schleihauf H, Kayhan E, Matthes D, Vrtička P, Hoehl S. Neural synchrony in mother–child conversation: exploring the role of conversation patterns. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2021;16:93–102.
Nguyen T, Zimmer L, Hoehl S. Your turn, my turn. Neural synchrony in mother–infant proto-conversation. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2023;378:20210488.
Wass SV, Whitehorn M, Marriott Haresign I, Phillips E, Leong V. Interpersonal neural entrainment during early social interaction. Trends Cogn Sci. 2020;24:329–42.
Sievers B, Welker C, Hasson U, Kleinbaum AM, Wheatley T. How consensus-building conversation changes our minds and aligns our brains. Psychology. 2020. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/562z7.
Kuelz A, Boyd S, Butler E. Close TIES in relationships: a dynamic systems approach for modeling physiological linkage. J Soc Pers Relatsh. 2022;39:3059–84.
Feldman R, Magori-Cohen R, Galili G, Singer M, Louzoun Y. Mother and infant coordinate heart rhythms through episodes of interaction synchrony. Infant Behav Dev. 2011;34:569–77.
Bell MA. Mother–child behavioral and physiological synchrony. In: Advances in child development and behavior. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2020. pp. 163–88.
Gordon I, Tomashin A, Mayo O A. Theory of flexible multimodal synchrony. arXiv:10.31234/osf.io/9u7q8 [Preprint]. 2023. Available from: https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/9u7q8
Feldman R. Infant-mother and infant-father synchrony: the coregulation of positive arousal. Infant Ment Health J. 2003;24:1–23.
Tronick EZ. Emotions and emotional communication in infants. Am Psychol. 1989;44:112–9.
Feldman R. Parent–infant synchrony: biological foundations and developmental outcomes. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2007;16:340–5.
Markova G, Nguyen T. Interpersonal synchrony is associated with infants’ reactions to subtle changes in caregiver‐infant interactions. Soc Dev. 2023;32:581–97.
Wass S, Phillips E, Haresign IM, Amadó MP, Goupil L. Contingency and synchrony: interactional pathways towards attentional control and intentional communication [Preprint]. 2024. Available from: https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/um4db
Hoehl S, Fairhurst M, Schirmer A. Interactional synchrony: signals, mechanisms and benefits. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2021;16:5–18.
Fuchs A, Lunkenheimer E, Brown K. Parental history of childhood maltreatment and child average RSA shape parent–child RSA synchrony. Dev Psychobiol. 2021;63:e22171.
Hoyniak CP, Quiñones-Camacho LE, Camacho MC, Chin JH, Williams EM, Wakschlag LS, et al. Adversity is linked with decreased parent-child behavioral and neural synchrony. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2021;48:100937.
Giuliano RJ, Skowron EA, Berkman ET. Growth models of dyadic synchrony and mother–child vagal tone in the context of parenting at-risk. Biol Psychol. 2015;105:29–36.
Azhari A, Leck WQ, Gabrieli G, Bizzego A, Rigo P, Setoh P, et al. Parenting stress undermines mother–child brain-to-brain synchrony: a hyperscanning study. Sci Rep. 2019;9:11407.
Clearfield MW, Carter-Rodriguez A, Merali A-R, Shober R. The effects of SES on infant and maternal diurnal salivary cortisol output. Infant Behav Dev. 2014;37:298–304.
Creaven A, Skowron EA, Hughes BM, Howard S, Loken E. Dyadic concordance in mother and preschooler resting cardiovascular function varies by risk status. Dev Psychobiol. 2014;56:142–52.
McKay JM, Pickens J, Stewart AL. Inventoried and observed stress in parent-child interactions. Curr Psychol. 1996;15:223–34.
Nguyen T, Schleihauf H, Kayhan E, Matthes D, Vrtička P, Hoehl S. The effects of interaction quality on neural synchrony during mother–child problem solving. Cortex. 2020;124:235–49.
Suveg C, Shaffer A, Davis M. Family stress moderates relations between physiological and behavioral synchrony and child self‐regulation in mother–preschooler dyads. Dev Psychobiol. 2016;58:83–97.
Tarullo AR, St John AM, Meyer JS. Chronic stress in the mother-infant dyad: maternal hair cortisol, infant salivary cortisol and interactional synchrony. Infant Behav Dev. 2017;47:92–102.
Anthonysamy A, Zimmer-Gembeck MJ. Peer status and behaviors of maltreated children and their classmates in the early years of school. Child Abuse Negl. 2007;31:971–91.
Bolger KE, Patterson CJ. Developmental pathways from child maltreatment to peer rejection. Child Dev. 2001;72:549–68.
Kay C, Green J. Reactive attachment disorder following early maltreatment: systematic evidence beyond the institution. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2013;41:571–81.
Rutter M, Sonuga-Barke EJ, Beckett C, Castle J, Kreppner J, Kumsta R, et al. Deprivation-specific psychological patterns: effects of institutional deprivation. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 2010;75:i–253.
Zahn-Waxler C, Van Hulle C. Empathy, guilt, and depression. In: Oakley B, Knafo A, Madhavan G, Wilson DS, editors. Pathological Altruism. New York: Oxford University Press; 2011. pp. 322–44.
Keil J, Perren S, Schlesier-Michel A, Sticca F, Sierau S, Klein AM, et al. Getting less than their fair share: maltreated youth are hyper-cooperative yet vulnerable to exploitation in a public goods game. Dev Sci. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12765.
Jackson JC, Jong J, Bilkey D, Whitehouse H, Zollmann S, McNaughton C, et al. Synchrony and physiological arousal increase cohesion and cooperation in large naturalistic groups. Sci Rep. 2018;8:127.
Lang M, Bahna V, Shaver JH, Reddish P, Xygalatas D. Sync to link: endorphin-mediated synchrony effects on cooperation. Biol Psychol. 2017;127:191–7.
Frazier TW, Strauss ME, Steinhauer SR. Respiratory sinus arrhythmia as an index of emotional response in young adults. Psychophysiology. 2004;41:75–83.
Sigrist C, Mürner-Lavanchy I, Peschel SKV, Schmidt SJ, Kaess M, Koenig J. Early life maltreatment and resting-state heart rate variability: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2021;120:307–34.
Miller JG, Armstrong‐Carter E, Balter L, Lorah J. A meta‐analysis of mother–child synchrony in respiratory sinus arrhythmia and contextual risk. Dev Psychobiol. 2023;65:e22355.
Lunkenheimer E, Busuito A, Brown KM, Skowron EA. Mother–child coregulation of parasympathetic processes differs by child maltreatment severity and subtype. Child Maltreat. 2018;23:211–20.
Buisman RSM, Bakermans‐Kranenburg MJ, Pittner K, Compier‐de Block LHCG, Van Den Berg LJM, Van IJzendoorn MH, et al. Parents’ experiences of childhood abuse and neglect are differentially associated with behavioral and autonomic responses to their offspring. Dev Psychobiol. 2019;61:888–902.
Sheridan MA, McLaughlin KA. Dimensions of early experience and neural development: deprivation and threat. Trends Cogn Sci. 2014;18:580–5.
Ellis BJ, Sheridan MA, Belsky J, McLaughlin KA. Why and how does early adversity influence development? Toward an integrated model of dimensions of environmental experience. Dev Psychopathol. 2022;34:447–71.
Alink LRA, Cyr C, Madigan S. The effect of maltreatment experiences on maltreating and dysfunctional parenting: a search for mechanisms. Dev Psychopathol. 2019;31:1–7.
Thomas LI, Fuchs R, Klaperski S. High trait emotional intelligence in men: beneficial for perceived stress levels but disadvantageous for the physiological response to acute stressors? J Appl Biobehav Res. 2018;23:e12116.
Bertsch K, Schmidinger I, Neumann ID, Herpertz SC. Reduced plasma oxytocin levels in female patients with borderline personality disorder. Horm Behav. 2013;63:424–9.
Müller LE, Bertsch K, Bülau K, Herpertz SC, Buchheim A. Emotional neglect in childhood shapes social dysfunctioning in adults by influencing the oxytocin and the attachment system: results from a population-based study. Int J Psychophysiol. 2019;136:73–80.
Gruhn MA, Compas BE. Effects of maltreatment on coping and emotion regulation in childhood and adolescence: a meta-analytic review. Child Abuse Negl. 2020;103:104446.
Suveg C, Braunstein West K, Davis M, Caughy M, Smith EP, Oshri A. Symptoms and synchrony: mother and child internalizing problems moderate respiratory sinus arrhythmia concordance in mother–preadolescent dyads. Dev Psychol. 2019;55:366–76.
Paladino M-P, Mazzurega M, Pavani F, Schubert TW. Synchronous multisensory stimulation blurs self–other boundaries. Psychol Sci. 2010;21:1202–7.
Hove MJ. Shared circuits, shared time, and interpersonal synchrony. Behav Brain Sci. 2008;31:29–30.
Smith ER An embodied account of self–other ‘overlap’ and its effects. In: Embodied grounding: social, cognitive, affective, and neuroscientific approaches. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2008. pp. 148–59.
Tarr B, Launay J. Dunbar RIM music and social bonding: “self–other” merging and neurohormonal mechanisms. Front Psychol. 2014. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01096.
Fonagy P, Gergely G, Target M. The parent–infant dyad and the construction of the subjective self. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2007;48:288–328.
Likens AD, Wiltshire TJ. Windowed multiscale synchrony: modeling time-varying and scale-localized interpersonal coordination dynamics. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2021;16:232–45.
Wass S, Greenwood E, Esposito G, Smith C, Necef I, Phillips E. Annual Research Review: ‘there, the dance is—at the still point of the turning world’—dynamic systems perspectives on coregulation and dysregulation during early development. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2024;65:481–507.
Abney DH, Dale R, Louwerse MM, Kello CT. The bursts and lulls of multimodal interaction: temporal distributions of behavior reveal differences between verbal and non‐verbal communication. Cogn Sci. 2018;42:1297–316.
Yoo H, Bowman DA, Oller DK. The origin of protoconversation: an examination of caregiver responses to cry and speech-like vocalizations. Front Psychol. 2018;9:1510.
Beebe B, Steele M. How does microanalysis of mother–infant communication inform maternal sensitivity and infant attachment? Attach Hum Dev. 2013;15:583–602.
Fonagy P. An open door review of outcome studies in psychoanalysis. Second revised ed. London: International psychoanalytical association; 2002.
De Bézenac CE, Swindells RA, Corcoran R. The necessity of ambiguity in self–other processing: a psychosocial perspective with implications for mental health. Front Psychol. 2018;9:2114.
Fotopoulou A, Tsakiris M. Mentalizing homeostasis: the social origins of interoceptive inference. Neuropsychoanalysis. 2017;19:3–28.
Kruse J, Walper S. Types of individuation in relation to parents: predictors and outcomes. Int J Behav Dev. 2008;32:390–400.
Ingoglia S, Lo Coco A, Liga F, Grazia Lo Cricchio M. Emotional separation and detachment as two distinct dimensions of parent—adolescent relationships. Int J Behav Dev. 2011;35:271–81.
Erikson EH. Identity: youth and crisis. Oxford: Norton & Co; 1968.
Olver RR, Aries E, Batgos J. Self–other differentiation and the mother–child relationship: the effects of sex and birth order. J Genet Psychol. 1989;150:311–22.
Quesque F, Brass M. The role of the temporoparietal junction in self–other distinction. Brain Topogr. 2019;32:943–55.
Steinbeis N. The role of self–other distinction in understanding others’ mental and emotional states: neurocognitive mechanisms in children and adults. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2016;371:20150074.
Uddin LQ, Iacoboni M, Lange C, Keenan JP. The self and social cognition: the role of cortical midline structures and mirror neurons. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11:153–7.
Bukowski H, Lamm C. Temporoparietal junction. In: Zeigler-Hill V, Shackelford TK, editors. Encyclopedia of personality and individual differences. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2017. pp 1–5.
Grosse Wiesmann C, Schreiber J, Singer T, Steinbeis N, Friederici AD. White matter maturation is associated with the emergence of theory of mind in early childhood. Nat Commun. 2017;8:14692.
Neisser U. Criteria for an ecological self. In: Advances in psychology. Elsevier; 1995. pp. 17–34.
Hakuno Y, Pirazzoli L, Blasi A, Johnson MH, Lloyd-Fox S. Optical imaging during toddlerhood: brain responses during naturalistic social interactions. Neurophotonics. 2018;5:1.
Lloyd-Fox S, Begus K, Halliday D, Pirazzoli L, Blasi A, Papademetriou M, et al. Cortical specialisation to social stimuli from the first days to the second year of life: a rural Gambian cohort. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2017;25:92–104.
Lamm C, Bukowski H, Silani G. From shared to distinct self–other representations in empathy: evidence from neurotypical function and socio–cognitive disorders. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2016;371:20150083.
Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3:201–15.
Serences JT, Shomstein S, Leber AB, Golay X, Egeth HE, Yantis S. Coordination of voluntary and stimulus-driven attentional control in human cortex. Psychol Sci. 2005;16:114–22.
Lotter LD, Kohl SH, Gerloff C, Bell L, Niephaus A, Kruppa JA, et al. Revealing the neurobiology underlying interpersonal neural synchronization with multimodal data fusion. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2023;146:105042.
Morgan JK, Santosa H, Conner KK, Fridley RM, Forbes EE, Iyengar S, et al. Mother–child neural synchronization is time linked to mother–child positive affective state matching. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2023;18:nsad001.
Luyten P, De Meulemeester C, Fonagy P. The self–other distinction in psychopathology: recent developments from a mentalizing perspective. In: Gilead M, Ochsner KN, editors. The neural basis of mentalizing. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2021. pp 659–80.
Machorrinho J, Veiga G, Santos G, Marmeleira J. Battered body, battered self: a cross-sectional study of the embodiment-related impairments of female victims of intimate partner violence. J Aggress Maltreatment Trauma. 2023;32:1589–603.
Botvinick M, Cohen J. Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see. Nature. 1998;391:756–756.
Talmon A, Ginzburg K. Between childhood maltreatment and shame: the roles of self-objectification and disrupted body boundaries. Psychol Women Q. 2017;41:325–37.
Maier A, Gieling C, Heinen-Ludwig L, Stefan V, Schultz J, Güntürkün O, et al. Association of childhood maltreatment with interpersonal distance and social touch preferences in adulthood. Am J Psychiatry. 2020;177:37–46.
Vranic A. Personal space in physically abused children. Environ Behav. 2003;35:550–65.
Obada OS. Body ego and trauma as correlates of comfort in the physical proximity of others. Pol Psychol Bull. 2014;45:92–100.
Pears KC, Fisher PA. Emotion understanding and theory of mind among maltreated children in foster care: evidence of deficits. Dev Psychopathol. 2005. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579405050030.
Benarous X, Guilé J-M, Consoli A, Cohen D. A systematic review of the evidence for impaired cognitive theory of mind in maltreated children. Front Psychiatry. 2015. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00108.
Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA, Maughan A, Toth SL, Bruce J. False belief understanding in maltreated children. Dev Psychopathol. 2003;15:1067–91.
Wong AE, Dirghangi SR, Hart SR. Self-concept clarity mediates the effects of adverse childhood experiences on adult suicide behavior, depression, loneliness, perceived stress, and life distress. Self Identity. 2019;18:247–66.
Burack JA, Flanagan T, Peled T, Sutton H, Zygmuntowicz C, Manly JT. Social perspective-taking skills in maltreated children and adolescents. Dev Psychol. 2006;42:207–17. 2
Hudson AR, De Coster L, Spoormans H, Verbeke S, Van der Jeught K, Brass M, et al. Childhood abuse and adult sociocognitive skills: distinguishing between self and other following early trauma. J Interpers Violence. 2021;36:NP13254–NP13274.
Fett AJ, Shergill SS, Gromann PM, Dumontheil I, Blakemore S, Yakub F, et al. Trust and social reciprocity in adolescence—a matter of perspective‐taking. J Adolesc. 2014;37:175–84.
Cracco E, Hudson AR, Van Hamme C, Maeyens L, Brass M, Mueller SC. Early interpersonal trauma reduces temporoparietal junction activity during spontaneous mentalising. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2020;15:12–22.
Boccadoro S, Cracco E, Hudson AR, Bardi L, Nijhof AD, Wiersema JR, et al. Defining the neural correlates of spontaneous theory of mind (ToM): an fMRI multi-study investigation. Neuroimage. 2019;203:116193.
Cheng TW, Mills KL, Miranda Dominguez O, Zeithamova D, Perrone A, Sturgeon D, et al. Characterizing the impact of adversity, abuse, and neglect on adolescent amygdala resting-state functional connectivity. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2021;47:100894.
Hudson A, Van Hamme C, Maeyens L, Brass M, Mueller S. Spontaneous mentalizing after early interpersonal trauma: evidence for hypoactivation of the temporoparietal junction. Neuroscience. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1101/487363.
Quidé Y, Ong XH, Mohnke S, Schnell K, Walter H, Carr VJ, et al. Childhood trauma-related alterations in brain function during a theory-of-mind task in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2017;189:162–8.
Roeckner AR, Oliver KI, Lebois LAM, Van Rooij SJH, Stevens JS. Neural contributors to trauma resilience: a review of longitudinal neuroimaging studies. Transl Psychiatry. 2021;11:508.
González-García N, Buimer EEL, Moreno-López L, Sallie SN, Váša F, Lim S, et al. Resilient functioning is associated with altered structural brain network topology in adolescents exposed to childhood adversity. Dev Psychopathol. 2023;35:1–11.
Crone EA, Dahl RE. Understanding adolescence as a period of social–affective engagement and goal flexibility. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012;13:636–50.
Walsh CM, Neff LA. We’re better when we blend: the benefits of couple identity fusion. Self Identity. 2018;17:587–603.
Viding E, McCrory E, Baskin-Sommers A, De Brito S, Frick P. An ‘embedded brain’ approach to understanding antisocial behaviour. Trends Cogn Sci. 2023. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2023.08.013.
Mayo O, Gordon I. In and out of synchrony—behavioral and physiological dynamics of dyadic interpersonal coordination. Psychophysiology. 2020;57:e13574.
Reindl V, Wass S, Leong V, Scharke W, Wistuba S, Wirth CL, et al. Multimodal hyperscanning reveals that synchrony of body and mind are distinct in mother–child dyads. Neuroimage. 2022;251:118982.
Santiesteban I, Banissy MJ, Catmur C, Bird G. Enhancing social ability by stimulating right temporoparietal junction. Curr Biol. 2012;22:2274–7.
Järvelä S, Salminen M, Ruonala A, Timonen J, Mannermaa K, Ravaja N, et al. DYNECOM: augmenting empathy in VR with dyadic synchrony neurofeedback. In: Proceedings of the 52nd Hawaii international conference on system sciences (HICSS). 2019. https://doi.org/10.24251/HICSS.2019.509.
Stepanova ER, Desnoyers-Stewart J, Pasquier P, Riecke BE. JeL: breathing together to connect with others and nature. In: Proceedings of the 2020 ACM designing interactive systems conference. Eindhoven Netherlands: ACM; 2020. pp. 641–54.
Santiesteban I, Banissy MJ, Catmur C, Bird G. Functional lateralization of temporoparietal junction—imitation inhibition, visual perspective‐taking and theory of mind. Eur J Neurosci. 2015;42:2527–33.
Yang L, Zhang W, Wang W, Yang Z, Wang H, Deng Z, et al. Neural and psychological predictors of cognitive enhancement and impairment from neurostimulation. Adv Sci. 2020;7:1902863.
Kohl SH, Melies P, Uttecht J, Lührs M, Bell L, Mehler DMA, et al. Successful modulation of temporoparietal junction activity and stimulus-driven attention by fNIRS-based neurofeedback—a randomized controlled proof-of-concept study. Imaging Neurosci. 2023;1:1–26.
Cirelli LK, Einarson KM, Trainor LJ. Interpersonal synchrony increases prosocial behavior in infants. Dev Sci. 2014;17:1003–11.
Reddish P, Fischer R, Bulbulia J. Let’s dance together: synchrony, shared intentionality and cooperation. PLoS One. 2013;8:e71182.
Bowling DL. Biological principles for music and mental health. Transl Psychiatry. 2023;13:374.
Czeszumski A, Liang SH-Y, Dikker S, König P, Lee C-P, Koole SL, et al. Cooperative behavior evokes inter-brain synchrony in the prefrontal and temporoparietal cortex: a systematic review and meta-analysis of fNIRS hyperscanning studies. eNeuro. 2021. https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.06.03.446922.
Dikker S, Michalareas G, Oostrik M, Serafimaki A, Kahraman HM, Struiksma ME, et al. Crowdsourcing neuroscience: Inter-brain coupling during face-to-face interactions outside the laboratory. Neuroimage. 2021;227:117436.
Konrad K, Gerloff C, Kohl SH, Mehler DMA, Mehlem L, Volbert EL, et al. Interpersonal neural synchrony and mental disorders: unlocking potential pathways for clinical interventions. Front Neurosci. 2024;18:1286130.
Endevelt-Shapira Y, Djalovski A, Dumas G, Feldman R. Maternal chemosignals enhance infant-adult brain-to-brain synchrony. Sci Adv. 2021;7:eabg6867.
Mu Y, Guo C, Han S. Oxytocin enhances inter-brain synchrony during social coordination in male adults. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2016;11:1882–93.
Spengler FB, Scheele D, Marsh N, Kofferath C, Flach A, Schwarz S, et al. Oxytocin facilitates reciprocity in social communication. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2017;12:1325–33.
Fiskum C. Psychotherapy beyond all the words: dyadic expansion, vagal regulation, and biofeedback in psychotherapy. J Psychother Integr. 2019;29:412–25.
Tennant JM, Cook S, Moldoveanu MC, Peterson JB, Cunningham WA. Interpersonal resonance: developing interpersonal biofeedback for the promotion of empathy and social entrainment. In: Ahram TZ, editor. Advances in human factors in wearable technologies and game design. Cham; Springer International Publishing; 2019. pp. 208–14.
Feijt MA, Okel SE, Ijsselsteijn WA. The effect of simulated feedback about psychophysiological synchronization on perceived empathy and connectedness.
Salminen M, Jarvela S, Ruonala A, Harjunen VJ, Hamari J, Jacucci G, et al. Evoking physiological synchrony and empathy using social VR with biofeedback. IEEE Trans Affect Comput. 2022;13:746–55.
Szymanski C, Müller V, Brick TR, Von Oertzen T, Lindenberger U. Hyper-transcranial alternating current stimulation: experimental manipulation of inter-brain synchrony. Front Hum Neurosci. 2017;11:539.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
KK and VP jointly conceived the work, reviewed and interpreted the existing data, and drafted and revised the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modified the licensed material. You do not have permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Konrad, K., Puetz, V.B. A context-dependent model of resilient functioning after childhood maltreatment—the case for flexible biobehavioral synchrony. Transl Psychiatry 14, 388 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-024-03092-7
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-024-03092-7