Abstract
Variation in the efficiency of extracting calorie-rich and nutrient-dense resources directly impacts energy expenditure and potentially has important repercussions for cultural transmission where social learning strategies are used. Assessing variation in efficiency is key to understanding the evolution of complex behavioural traits in primates. Here we examine evidence for individual-level differences beyond age- and sex-class in non-human primate extractive foraging efficiency. We used 25 years (1992–2017) of video of 21 chimpanzees aged ≥6 years in Bossou, Guinea, to longitudinally investigate individual-level differences in stone tool use efficiency. Data from 3,882 oil-palm nut-cracking bouts from >800 h of observation were collected. We found reliability in relative efficiency across four measures of nut-cracking efficiency, as well as a significant effect of age. Our findings highlight the importance of longitudinal data from long-term field sites when investigating underlying cognitive and behavioural diversity across individual lifespans and between populations.
Similar content being viewed by others
Main
The importance of individual variation in cognition and behaviour is increasingly appreciated in research on non-human animals1. Such variation can have ramifications at both the individual and population levels, with broader implications for life history2,3,4, cultural evolution and interpretations of the archaeological record5 (Fig. 1). For example, a recent large-scale meta-analysis found individual variation in the migration timing of land-, water- and seabirds, which impacts the breeding success and survival of a given migration6.
The schematic illustrates the implications of long-term individual differences in stone tool use on four major domains (orange): the potential presence of social learning strategies, the generation of behavioural innovations, the interpretation of the archaeological record, and the presence of variation in daily energy budgets and expenditure. In turn these have four broader evolutionary implications (green): transmission via social learning, cultural evolution, technological evolution, and life history strategies and evolutionary fitness. Arrows indicate the relationships between the major domains and the broader evolutionary implications.
Yet, although cognitive and behavioural variation has been reported, the research has mostly focused on one time point for each individual or is limited to short time spans1,7,8. We argue that this approach does not reflect the true extent and patterns of variation within wild populations. Here we aim to fill this knowledge gap, investigating individual differences in chimpanzee technological efficiency and their persistence over time.
First, persistent variation in the proficiency and efficiency with which individuals extract high-quality resources from the environment directly impacts energy expenditure and individual fitness. An individual who is slow or inefficient at a given extractive-foraging technique relative to others in the population will have less time and energy for other fitness-enhancing tasks, thus incurring relative fitness costs9. This is particularly true for complex tool-assisted foraging tasks aimed at extracting high-calorie resources, such as wood-boring beetle larvae extraction by New Caledonian crows (Corvus moneduloides)10, oyster-cracking by Burmese long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis aurea)7, and honey-dipping, termite-fishing and nut-cracking by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes spp.)8,11,12. Understanding the extent and causes of individual differences in extractive-foraging tasks is key for identifying variation in factors known to be relevant to evolutionary fitness.
Second, there is growing recognition that variation in tool use produces variation in the traces left in the archaeological record, particularly for the signatures produced during percussive behaviours5. For example, the number of unintentional flakes produced by wild bearded capuchin monkeys (Sapajus libidinosus) while cracking nuts with stones will vary according to the tool user, with the skill (or lack thereof) of the individual determining the frequency of mishits and subsequent flakes13. Investigating the archaeological signatures of known individuals can inform our understanding of hominin fossil sites and the context behind technological traces5. However, there is currently no research on individual-level variation in chimpanzee stone tool use, constituting a substantial gap in our understanding of primate technological behaviour and its potential implications for hominin tool use.
Third, identifying systematic variation in technological behaviour is crucial for investigating the factors influencing individual differences in behavioural acquisition via social learning. Indeed, although there are species-typical forms of social learning for particular skills, there is growing evidence that social learning mechanisms vary systematically across individuals14. Individual-level variation can develop from ‘individual learning of social learning’, whereby differing experiences with previous social learning opportunities can result in different strategies being used in the future, or the rate of learning being altered. This phenotypic plasticity can facilitate faster adaptation to environmental changes, potentially having profound effects on evolutionary processes. This is particularly true where model selection and social learning biases guide behavioural acquisition15,16,17,18. For example, female migrant chimpanzees in the Taï Forest, Côte d’Ivoire, conform to the nut-cracking technique of their new community, even when their previous technique was more efficient (in terms of strikes per nut and foraging speed)17. Therefore, assessing variation contributes to our understanding of cultural transmission and the evolution of complex behavioural traits in primates19.
Variation in technological behaviour in non-human primates is well established. For example, only male bearded capuchins use sticks as probing tools20. In chimpanzees, inter- and intra-population variation in tool use is found across all communities, including differences in technological strategies and efficiency. For instance, ant-dipping tool lengths differ between the two neighbouring communities in the Kalinzu Forest, Uganda21. Chimpanzees in Kibale National Park, Uganda, used sticks to extract experimentally introduced honey, whereas chimpanzees in Budongo National Park, Uganda, either used their fingers or leaf sponges22. Chimpanzees in Loango National Park, Gabon, use different grip types to perforate nests during honey extraction, which is unaffected by the soil hardness8. In Gombe National Park, Tanzania, females commence termite-dipping earlier, engage in fishing more frequently and retrieve more termites per dip than males23.
In nut-cracking, females in Taï crack Coula edulis and Panda oleosa nuts more efficiently (measured as the mean number of strikes per nut and nuts per minute) than males24,25. By contrast, males in Bossou, Guinea, select, use and transport tools more frequently than females26, and spend a significantly greater proportion of their time cracking oil palm (Elaeis guineensis) nuts compared with adult females27. The number of hits required to successfully open the nutshell was previously found to reach an asymptote in adulthood28, and the movement of adult crackers was considered to be stereotyped29. However, this finding is constrained to a measure of strikes per nut, which is only one measure of nut-cracking efficiency. Moreover, no research has sought to replicate this finding, and the presence and stability of variation in nut-cracking behaviour has not been investigated using long-term data.
The combination of chimpanzees’ long lifespans and the protracted learning periods (3–5 years for nut-cracking in Bossou29,30) required to learn complex tool use necessitates longitudinal studies for assessing developmental trajectories and social learning. Although cross-sectional data can be useful for determining general developmental milestones, they do not allow for tests of individual variation across the lifespan. By obscuring potential variability, this may result in generalizations about population- or species-typical development of a behavioural trait. Tracking the development of a skill across an individual’s life course is key for understanding the ontogenetic processes involved—and potential variation in—fitness-enhancing traits, including extractive technologies.
This study assessed five measures (Table 1) of post-early learning period (hereafter, ‘post-ELP’) nut-cracking efficiency for 21 individuals present in a 25-year video archive of wild chimpanzees. ‘Post-ELP’ was defined as chimpanzees aged 6 and over, based on the previously established period of social learning lasting until 5 years old29. Data were gathered from all post-ELP individuals present in the archive for each year they were in the footage (Fig. 2 and Supplementary Table 1). This research takes a longitudinal approach to investigate the presence of variation in primate extractive foraging. We also assess the reliability (internal consistency across efficiency measures) and stability (whether individual differences hold over time)1 of such differences in a wild primate population.
Results
Nut-cracking bouts
Bouts are the continuous periods (in seconds) of nut-cracking whereby the individual strikes a single nut on an anvil with a hammer stone involving the same hand, bodily posture, hand grip and nut31. A total of 3,882 oil-palm nut-cracking bouts were recorded across 21 chimpanzees (12 females and 9 males; ages 6–60) over a 25-year period. Of these bouts, 318 (8.17%) ended in failure (no kernel extracted), 336 (8.66%) ended with a smashed kernel (only partial retrieval of the kernel) and 3,228 (83.17%) ended in the successful retrieval of a whole kernel.
Individual variation
Analyses (see Supplementary Tables 2–6 for full model outputs) revealed that including random intercepts for individual chimpanzees improved model fit for all measures: log bout duration (χ2(1) = 369.49, P < 0.0001), strikes per nut (χ2(1) = 475.26, P < 0.0001), success rate (χ2(1) = 47.16, P < 0.0001), displacement rate (χ2(1) = 87.022, P < 0.0001) and tool switch rate (χ2(1) = 4.17, P = 0.0411). Age had a significant positive fixed effect on log bout duration (t(1400) = 7.724, P < 0.001, β = 0.02, 95% confidence interval (CI) = (0.016, 0.028)) and strikes per nut (z(3362) = 8.51, P < 0.001, β = 0.024, 95% CI = (0.019, 0.030)), and a significant negative fixed effect on displacement rate (z(3666) = −2.40, P = 0.0164, β = −0.012, 95% CI = (−0.022, −0.002)) and tool switch rate (z(3666) = −2.462, P = 0.0138, β = −0.010, 95% CI = (−0.018, −0.002)). There was a significant fixed effect of sex for tool switch rate (male, z(3666) = −2.502, P = 0.0124, β = −0.384, 95% CI = (−0.685, −0.083)), but no significant fixed effect of sex for log bout duration (male, t(16) = 0.238, P = 0.812, β = 0.07, 95% CI = (−0.520, 0.670)), strikes per nut (male, z(3362) = 0.407, P = 0.684, β = 0.114, 95% CI = (−0.436, 0.664)), success rate (male, z(3668) = −0.1296, P = 0.897, β = −0.025, 95% CI = (−0.406, 0.356)) or displacement rate (male, z(3666) = −1.029, P = 0.304, β = −0.211, 95% CI = (−0.612, 0.190)). Data distributions for each individual across the five efficiency measures can be found in Supplementary Figs. 1–5.
Reliability of individual variation
Next, we ranked the random intercepts for each multilevel model; random intercepts represent estimated individual-level effects on the outcome variable in the model. Lower ranks represent greater relative nut-cracking efficiency: fewer strikes per nut, shorter bout durations and so on. Ranks ranged from 1 to 21 (the total number of individuals in the dataset; Fig. 3). Individual ranks for log bout duration, strikes per nut, success rate and displacement rate were strongly correlated (mean correlation, r = 0.718; Supplementary Fig. 6), meaning that when an individual was ranked highly on one of these measures, they also ranked highly in the other three. However, tool switch rate was only moderately correlated (mean correlation, r = 0.319), suggesting that it reflects a different underlying construct.
The consistency of the relative rank of each individual’s random intercepts obtained in the log bout duration, strikes per nut, success rate and displacement rate models indicates reliable individual differences in nut-cracking efficiency. A two-way intra-class correlation (ICC) analysis also indicated good agreement, F(20,60) = 11.2, P < 0.001, ICC(A,1) = 0.728, 0.556 < ICC < 0.863. Including tool switch rate in the two-way ICC decreased the internal consistency of the efficiency measures (F(20,80) = 7.32, P < 0.001, ICC(A,1) = 0.57, 0.377 < ICC < 0.76), further suggesting that it reflects a different underlying construct.
Stability of inter-individual relative efficiency
We also assessed whether individuals’ relative nut-cracking efficiency was stable over time. To do this, we plotted the data and random slopes for the effect of age on the outcome of interest for individuals who had at least 3 years of data during what we call the ‘adult proficiency period’ (hereafter, APP): the period from ages 11 to 40. The APP encompasses the period of adulthood from maturity to the start of declining efficiency owing to old age (40 years old; Howard-Spink, E., Matsuzawa, T., Carvalho, S., Hobaiter, C., Almeida-Warren, K. et al., unpublished manuscript)32 and also reflects the continued intra-individual improvement in efficiency from ages 6 to 10 before maturity is reached (Supplementary Figs. 1–5). By looking at individuals with at least 3 years of data, we were able to determine whether the random slope lines intersected one another or not, independent of the effects of age on efficiency reported above. In other words, we asked: even if individual X’s efficiency improved or worsened over time, were they always more or less efficient than individual Y?
This dataset (n = 7; 3 males, 4 females; between 1,047 and 1,123 bouts in total, depending on the outcome measure) spans the APP, with a younger cohort of two individuals (1 male, 1 female, aged 11–20), one individual across the middle period (male, aged 12–37) and an older cohort of four individuals (1 male, 3 females, aged 34–40). The individuals in the younger cohort were both born in 1997 and their APP data spans 2008–2017, and the older cohort were all a similar age at the start of the study period (32–36 years old in 1992) and had APP data from 1992 to 1999. The middle individual was born in 1980 and did not overlap in age with any other individual during the same years in our dataset. The presence of multiple individuals of the same or similar ages overlapping in time allows for direct comparisons of relative efficiency within these two cohorts.
We were able to create models with random slopes for age for strikes per nut and displacement rate (models for log bout duration and success rate did not converge or had issues with singularity, respectively). The results are shown in Fig. 4. Although more data are needed for inferential statistics, we note that estimated random slopes for individuals within age cohorts do not intersect, suggesting little change in relative efficiency over time within these groups.
Inter-rater reliability
Two independent, hypothesis-blind coders reviewed 70 h (8.41%) of footage for inter-coder reliability analyses. Unweighted Cohen’s κ and ICC analyses indicated substantial–excellent agreement and consistency between coders (Supplementary Table 7).
Discussion
This longitudinal research establishes reliable individual differences in efficiency across four measures of nut-cracking technological efficiency in wild chimpanzees. This finding highlights the necessity to move beyond the exclusive use of group averages when investigating daily energy expenditure and activity budgets—factors with important implications for life history and foraging ecology in primates. Variation in the time and energy that an individual expends on nut-cracking could produce variation in resource allocation to other fitness-enhancing traits. Thus, the extent of variation in nut-cracking efficiency may have large implications for other factors impacting survival and reproduction at the individual level, such as social learning, as well as the evolution of cultural traits.
Moreover, this research demonstrates individual-level variation in chimpanzee stone tool use. This finding reiterates the need to consider variation in the archaeological signatures left by percussive technological behaviours, with some individuals potentially contributing more to the record than others5,33. Research is now needed to ascertain the sources of this variation. A particular focus on the nut-cracking learning period is key given the known developmental drivers of variation in technological behaviour23,34,35.
The ranked random intercepts for log bout duration, strikes per nut, success rate and displacement rate were closely correlated, but the raw scores were not perfectly correlated. For example, the correlation between the ranked random intercepts for log bout duration and strikes per nut was greater than the correlation between their raw scores (r = 0.99 and r = 0.756, respectively). Indeed, longer bouts do not necessarily equate to more strikes per nut, with bout duration also being extended owing to factors such as greater distractibility or taking longer pauses between strikes. This supports the view that each measure captures distinct, but internally consistent, aspects of what can be termed ‘efficiency’. Bout duration is an indicator of energy intake, as shorter bouts allow more kernels to be retrieved over the course of a feeding session. This may improve an individual’s food security, as they will be at a competitive advantage compared with conspecifics in terms of access to more nuts. Also, longer bouts mean that the individual’s attention is focused on the task for a relatively longer period of time, such that there is reduced time to be allocated to other fitness-enhancing behaviours such as defence, socialization and grooming.
Conversely, the number of strikes required to retrieve the kernel and the success rate of the individual are indicators of energy expenditure relative to energy intake. The number of strikes per nut also dictates how convenient it is for the individual to use stones to open the nuts (rather than, for example, using their teeth or scrounging kernels), with more convenient techniques being favoured owing to their increased efficiency36. For success rate, future research should investigate why chimpanzees choose to end a nut-cracking bout before the kernel is extracted and assess whether certain individuals stop bouts earlier than others (that is, do not expend unnecessary energy and are hence more efficient).
Displacement rate measures the ability, or lack thereof, to judge the amount of kinetic energy required to strike a nut. Displacing the nut not only expends more energy than is required, but also elongates the bout duration, particularly if the individual has to travel to retrieve the nut. This also reduces the convenience of using tools. Primate archaeological research should investigate whether individuals with higher displacement rates are also more likely to fracture their stone tools, resulting in potential unintentional flakes13,37.
Our results also suggest that the number of times individuals adjust their tools during a bout reflects a different underlying process. Tool switch rate is a proxy measure for the individual’s ability to select an efficient tool composite and position the stones in such a way to reduce the energetic effort required to extract the kernel. Given the importance of tool properties in determining the efficiency of the behaviour17,38,39, learning to select the best tools is also an important aspect of successful foraging, although our results suggest that it does not relate to how efficient the individual is at cracking nuts per se.
Moreover, our results suggest that relative inter-individual nut-cracking efficiency (in terms of the number of strikes per nut and the displacement rate) for a subset of seven chimpanzees may be stable over time during the APP, although we call for more research on this topic using more robust datasets. Despite our data being relatively sparse—our models included only seven individuals—we were able to estimate the trajectory of individuals’ nut-cracking efficiency over time within age cohorts who overlapped in the years they were present in the Bossou archive. Model estimates suggest that an individual’s efficiency relative to others in its cohort persisted across overlapping ages; individuals’ predicted random slopes for the effects of age on the efficiency outcomes did not intersect. These model predictions are thus consistent with the assertion that individuals’ relative efficiency ranks remain stable over time. However, given the relatively small sample of individuals, these findings should be viewed as suggestive and an avenue for future research.
The presence of stable variation in the different measures of nut-cracking efficiency could indicate variation in the underlying cognitive and/or motor capacities. For example, longer bout durations may signify that certain individuals are prone to distraction rather than an individual being inherently less skilled. Therefore, the potential long-term stability in individual variation in nut-cracking efficiency may suggest that there are stable cognitive differences between the individuals, which potentially has implications for performance in other key fitness-enhancing behaviours. Future research should assess whether variation exists in other behaviours in the community to determine the extent of these cognitive differences and their contributions to fitness-relevant behaviours and outcomes.
This research did not seek to directly test whether increased nut-cracking efficiency corresponded to relative fitness gains as the presence of confounds hinders our ability to establish a causal link between tool use efficiency and longevity or reproductive success. For example, a shorter lifespan of a relatively less efficient individual could be the result of technological inefficiency or another factor that negatively impacts both tool use efficiency and longevity (for example, physical weakness or ill-health). Research specifically investigating the fitness consequences of individual differences in technological efficiency should consider the broader energy budgets and expenditure, as relatively inefficient nutcrackers may make up for their energy disadvantages through increased efficiency in other tasks.
Age had a significant fixed effect on log bout duration, strikes per nut, displacement rate and tool switch rate. Increasing age corresponds to longer bouts and more strikes per nut, which may reflect greater muscle weakness and the need for more rests in old age. Indeed, there have been many elderly chimpanzees in the Bossou population, with six individuals in the archive being aged 45+. Conversely, increasing age also corresponds to fewer nut displacements and fewer tool switches during bouts. This may reflect older individuals’ greater proficiency at selecting appropriate tools, and better ability at judging the amount of kinetic energy required for each strike. More research is needed to assess the impact of old age on chimpanzee technological capabilities.
Consistent with previous research demonstrating that an individual’s performance continues to improve until around age 10 (ref. 40), we show that an individual’s efficiency increases until they reach maturity at age 11. Performance often improves and becomes less variable at ages 11–20 compared with at ages 6–10, suggesting that learning extends beyond the originally conceived learning period lasting until around 5 years old29,41. Although the initial acquisition of the skill relies on social learning from knowledgeable group mates29, individuals’ proficiency is honed through practice during the sub-adult period.
Unlike previous research, we found inconsistent effects of sex on technological efficiency. Sex had no significant effect on log bout duration, strikes per nut, success rate or displacement rate, but there was a significant effect on tool switch rate, with male chimpanzees switching tools less frequently than female chimpanzees. This finding contradicts previous research establishing a female bias in technological behaviour across the genus Pan42. This may reflect circumstances specific to the Bossou chimpanzees. Indeed, Bossou is an isolated community that has been decreasing in size, with both factors potentially contributing to unusual patterns compared with other populations. The lack of sex differences in this research may suggest that the mechanism(s) establishing a female bias in nut-cracking efficiency in Taï24 are not present in Bossou. However, it may be that the apparent female bias is an artefact of short-term, cross-sectional analyses and that the effect does not hold longitudinally43. This reiterates the importance of conducting long-term investigations of long-lived primates as data from short-term studies may not be representative of behaviour over the lifespan. Further long-term research is required to determine whether this finding holds for other populations of nut-cracking chimpanzees.
The research is limited by three main constraints. First, the outdoor laboratory is a field experimental set-up, with locally sourced nuts and stones being provisioned, and the experiments only occurring during the dry season (when there is high fruit availability). It could be argued that this decreases the validity of the findings and that they may not reflect the year-round nut-cracking efficiency of the individuals (such as in periods of low fruit availability). However, the nut-cracking that occurs in the outdoor laboratory is no different from the nut-cracking that the Bossou chimpanzees perform at the natural cracking site of Moblim (07° 38′ 20.7″ N, 008° 30′ 39.2″ W)26. Moreover, the ___location of the outdoor laboratory was specifically selected to be on Mount Gban’s summit—the core of the home range for this chimpanzee community—to optimize the frequency of chimpanzees visiting the site40,44. This suggests that nut-cracking behaviour remains unchanged regardless of site ___location, reiterating the ecological validity of the outdoor laboratory.
Second, despite the experimental nature of the outdoor laboratory, there are risks of confounding variables. For example, there is no control for daily intake of energy from other sources or levels of physical activity, both of which could influence motivations for cracking nuts45. However, footage selected for analysis was randomly sampled, representing a period of many years, in an attempt to amplify the signal-to-noise ratio. As such, the dataset produced here represents a critical longitudinal insight into the technological behaviour of a community presently on the cusp of extinction.
Third, the Bossou population structure hindered the collection of data from multiple individuals for the 20–35-year-old age range. As such, data were only obtained from one chimpanzee (Foaf), despite this being a potentially key period of stability in technological efficiency. This sample size of one meant that we could not compare the performance of individuals in this age bracket. More generally, across all ages, there were very few individuals who overlapped in time, meaning that there were insufficient data for an inferential analysis of the stability of relative nut-cracking efficiency over time. Future research from field sites with larger community sizes is needed to further address the question of long-term stability of individual differences in technological efficiency.
Conclusion
This study systematically assessed individual differences in nut-cracking in the Bossou chimpanzees using a long-term video archive of 25 years to longitudinally investigate these differences. Our results suggest reliable individual-level differences across four measures of nut-cracking efficiency, shedding light on the underlying cognitive and behavioural diversity in the Bossou chimpanzees. This research contributes to a growing body of evidence finding stable and reliable cognitive abilities in great apes, and points to potential variation in the development of this extractive foraging skill. Future research should seek to establish the factors driving this individual variation and its development over time.
Methods
Ethical approval and permissions to conduct scientific research in the Bossou community were obtained by each contributor to the video archive from the Direction Générale de la Recherche Scientifique et de l’Innovation Technologique (DGERSIT) and the Institut de Recherche Environnementale de Bossou (IREB) in Guinea.
Study site
Bossou is a village in south-eastern Guinea (7° 38′ 71.7″ N, 8° 29′ 38.9″ W), with a tropical wet seasonal climate and a predominant population of the Manon ethnic group46. The neighbouring chimpanzee community resides in primary and secondary forest, with a home range of 15 km2, although their core area is 7 km2. The chimpanzees in Bossou have been studied continuously since 1976, with 21 individuals being present47. Since then, the community has been declining in size48. The Bossou chimpanzees use a stone hammer-and-anvil composite to extract oil palm nuts11,37,49, with individuals requiring complementary coordinated action of both hands to manoeuvre three objects (hammer, anvil and nut) during a nut-cracking bout50. The chimpanzees also crack experimentally introduced coula nuts29.
Study materials
The Bossou chimpanzees have been recorded in every dry season (December–February) since 1988, resulting in a long-term video archive of their behaviour. Researchers observed and videoed the chimpanzees in the ‘outdoor laboratory’29,40—a 7 m × 20 m clearing in the core of the community’s home range on Mount Gban (7° 38′ 41.5″ N, 8° 29′ 50.0″ W), which is passed through daily26,29,40. The outdoor laboratory is experimental in nature, with many of the available raw materials (with established weights and dimensions) and nuts being provisioned by the researchers37. Observations of behaviour within the outdoor laboratory occur from behind a grass screen along one edge of the clearing. All observation sessions were recorded using at least two standardized camera angles37 (wide- and standard-angle lenses), optimizing the viewing angles.
Measures
Five distinct aspects of nut-cracking efficiency were measured: (1) the time it took for one nut to be cracked open (bout duration), (2) the number of times the nut was hit with the hammer (strikes per nut), (3) the proportion of bouts ending in the whole kernel being extracted, a broken kernel being extracted or no kernel being extracted (success rate), (4) the number of times the hammer strike resulted in the nut being hit off the anvil (displacement rate) and (5) the number of times the hammer stone was changed or repositioned when trying to crack one nut (tool switch rate). The archival nature of the data meant that hammer size could not be controlled for; however, the Bossou chimpanzees select stone tools based on their properties and attribute functions to the stones based on those features (for example, hammer stones are wider and lighter than those used for anvils)26. As such, hammers fall into a limited range of standard sizes and so there is likely little variation in hammer size selection.
Data collection
Behavioural analysis was conducted using Behavioural Observation Research Interactive Software (BORIS, v. 7.11.1)51. Of the 1,185 videos in the Bossou archive from 1992 to 2017, 966 videos contained visible nut-cracking bouts by at least one individual, amounting to 832 observation hours. Where possible, the videos from the standard-angle lens data were used for analysis. If the behaviour was obscured in this footage, the equivalent video with the wide-angle lens was uploaded to attempt to observe the behaviour. If the behaviour remained unobservable, the bout(s) were excluded. No footage was collected in 2001 or 2011, and so these are absent from the dataset.
Data were collected for all post-ELP (n = 21) chimpanzees present during the study period, with bouts being recorded for each year each focal individual was cracking nuts. Multiple bouts (up to 20) per individual per year were recorded to establish the degree of within-individual variation in efficiency, while also producing more independent data points, allowing between-individual variation to be assessed. No statistical methods were used to pre-determine the sample size, but our sample size is similar to those reported in previous publications (refs. 1,8). The full data collection protocol can be found on page 13 of Supplementary Information.
Statistical analysis
Data were collected from a total of 4,188 nut-cracking bouts. All bouts where coula nuts were cracked (n = 281) were excluded so that only data from native oil palm nuts were analysed. All bouts where the bout outcome had not been recorded (n = 31) were excluded, leaving 3,882 complete oil-palm nut-cracking bouts for analysis. All bouts where an infant was clinging to the focal subject were removed from analyses (n = 210), as this was exclusive to certain female chimpanzees (Fana, Fanle, Fotaiu, Jire, Pili, Velu, Vuavua and Yo) and so could reduce the internal validity of the findings by altering the efficiency of these individuals. Removing these bouts ensured that all individuals were compared under equal circumstances. This left 3,672 bouts for analysis.
Only data for bouts in which a kernel was retrieved were included in the analyses for bout duration and strikes per nut (n = 3,367). Excluding the ‘Failed’ bouts here ensured that the amount of time it took to access the energetic reward of the enclosed kernel was analysed. The full dataset (n = 3,672) was analysed for success rate, displacement rate and tool switch rate.
All analyses were performed using R (v. 4.3.2)52 and RStudio (v. 2023.09.1+494) for MacOS. The significance level (α) was 0.05 for all analyses. Multilevel models were constructed to test for individual differences in the five measures of efficiency. Individual chimpanzees comprised the random factor, while age and sex were included as fixed effects. Simple models (without random effects) were constructed and compared against the multilevel models (Supplementary Tables 2–6); ANOVAs were used to determine the model with the best fit. The ANOVAs (two-tailed) assessed whether including a random intercept for individual chimpanzees significantly decreased the model prediction error. The model with the smallest prediction error (AIC and −2-log-likelihood values) was selected for each of the five components of efficiency. To assess stability of relative efficiency within age cohorts over time, random slopes for the effects of age were added to models, when possible (see below).
The linear multilevel model for bout duration was fitted and assessed using the lme453 and lmerTest54 packages, respectively. Initially, a simple linear multilevel model (subject as a random intercept) was constructed, with age and sex as fixed effects. The bout duration data were strongly right skewed, so it was log transformed for use in the linear multilevel model55. Random slopes for the effect of age were not possible in this model, as the model failed to converge when including this random effect structure.
Given that the count data for strikes per nut did not contain zeros, zero-truncated Poisson (multilevel) models were fitted using the truncated_poisson family in the glmmTMB package56, with age and sex as fixed effects. However, the check_dispersion function from the performance package57 detected overdispersion in the multilevel model (subject as a random intercept). As such, zero-truncated negative binomial (multilevel) models were fitted with quadratic parameterization. Random slopes for the effects of age were added to the model to assess stability of relative efficiency over time.
As the data for success rate were ordinal, cumulative link (multilevel) models were fitted using the ordinal package58. The outcome factor was ordered to Failed, Smash, Successful, to account for the increasing degrees of efficiency with each increasing level of outcome38. Hessian matrices were calculated for all models to calculate model summaries. The number of quadrature points used in the adaptive Gauss–Hermite quadrature approximation was set to 7. Age was originally included as a fixed effect but was removed as the model did not converge. Sex remained as a fixed effect. Random slopes for the effect of age were not possible in this model, as the model had a singularity issue when including this random effect structure.
Negative binomial (multilevel) models were fitted for displacement rate and tool switch rate using the glmmTMB package. All models specified zero-inflation as being equal for all observations, given the large number of zeros in the data where an individual did not displace the nut (n = 2,909) or switch their tools (n = 3,308) in a bout. Random slopes for the effects of age were added to the displacement rate model to assess stability of relative efficiency over time.
The individuals’ random intercepts were ranked for the log bout duration, strikes per nut, success rate, displacement rate and tool switch rate models, and a correlation matrix was constructed to determine whether they represent the same underlying construct. The two-way ICC used to assess the reliability of individual variation was selected according to recommended guidelines59.
Model assumptions were checked and revealed no issues (see page 17 of Supplementary Information and Supplementary Fig. 7).
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Data availability
All data can be found in the following public repository: https://osf.io/qw9ua.
Code availability
All analysis code needed to reproduce the results and figures reported in the paper and Supplementary Information can be found in the following public repository: https://github.com/arranjdavis/chimpanzee_nut_cracking_efficiency.
References
Bohn, M. et al. Great ape cognition is structured by stable cognitive abilities and predicted by developmental conditions. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 7, 927–938 (2023).
Pontzer, H. et al. Primate energy expenditure and life history. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 111, 1433–1437 (2014).
Kraft, T. S. et al. The energetics of uniquely human subsistence strategies. Science 374, eabf0130 (2021).
Kaplan, H. S., Hill, K. R., Lancaster, J. & Hurtado, A. M. A theory of human life history evolution: diet, intelligence, and longevity. Evol. Anthropol. 9, 156–185 (2000).
Pascual-Garrido, A., Carvalho, S. & Almeida-Warren, K. Primate archaeology 3.0. Am. J. Biol. Anthropol. 183, 1–17 (2023).
Franklin, K. A. et al. Individual repeatability of avian migration phenology: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Anim. Ecol. 91, 1416–1430 (2022).
Tan, A., Tan, S. H., Vyas, D., Malaivijitnond, S. & Gumert, M. D. There is more than one way to crack an oyster: identifying variation in Burmese long-tailed macaque (Macaca fascicularis aurea) stone-tool use. PLoS ONE 10, e0124733 (2015).
Estienne, V., Stephens, C. & Boesch, C. Extraction of honey from underground bee nests by central African chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes troglodytes) in Loango National Park, Gabon: techniques and individual differences. Am. J. Primatol. 79, e22672 (2017).
Biro, D., Haslam, M. & Rutz, C. Tool use as adaptation. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 368, 20120408 (2013).
Rutz, C. et al. The ecological significance of tool use in New Caledonian crows. Science 329, 1523–1526 (2010).
Matsuzawa, T. in The Chimpanzees of Bossou and Nimba (eds Matsuzawa, T. et al.) 73–84 (Springer, 2011).
Boesch, C. et al. Chimpanzee ethnography reveals unexpected cultural diversity. Nat. Hum. Behav. 4, 910–916 (2020).
Luncz, L. V., Arroyo, A., Falótico, T., Quinn, P. & Proffitt, T. A primate model for the origin of flake technology. J. Hum. Evol. 171, 103250 (2022).
Mesoudi, A., Chang, L., Dall, S. R. X. & Thornton, A. The evolution of individual and cultural variation in social learning. Trends Ecol. Evol. 31, 215–225 (2016).
Henrich, J. & Gil-White, F. J. The evolution of prestige: freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evol. Hum. Behav. 22, 165–196 (2001).
Kendal, R. L. et al. Chimpanzees copy dominant and knowledgeable individuals: implications for cultural diversity. Evol. Hum. Behav. 36, 65–72 (2015).
Luncz, L. V., Sirianni, G., Mundry, R. & Boesch, C. Costly culture: differences in nut-cracking efficiency between wild chimpanzee groups. Anim. Behav. 137, 63–73 (2018).
Tan, A., Hemelrijk, C. K., Malaivijitnond, S. & Gumert, M. D. Young macaques (Macaca fascicularis) preferentially bias attention towards closer, older, and better tool users. Anim. Cogn. 21, 551–5563 (2018).
Tan, A. From play to proficiency: the ontogeny of stone-tool use in coastal-foraging long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) from a comparative perception-action perspective. J. Comp. Psychol. 131, 89–114 (2017).
Falótico, T., Bueno, C. Q. & Ottoni, E. B. Ontogeny and sex differences in object manipulation and probe tool use by wild tufted capuchin monkeys (Sapajus libidinosus). Am. J. Primatol. 83, e23251 (2021).
Koops, K., Schöning, C., Isaji, M. & Hashimoto, C. Cultural differences in ant-dipping tool length between neighbouring chimpanzee communities at Kalinzu, Uganda. Sci. Rep. 5, 12456 (2015).
Gruber, T., Muller, M. N., Strimling, P., Wrangham, R. W. & Zuberbühler, K. Wild chimpanzees rely on cultural knowledge to solve an experimental honey acquisition task. Curr. Biol. 19, 1806–1810 (2009).
Lonsdorf, E. V. Sex differences in the development of termite-fishing skills in the wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii, of Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Anim. Behav. 70, 673–683 (2005).
Boesch, C. & Boesch, H. Sex differences in the use of natural hammers by wild chimpanzees: a preliminary report. J. Hum. Evol. 10, 585–593 (1981).
Boesch, C. & Boesch, H. Possible causes of sex differences in the use of natural hammers by wild chimpanzees. J. Hum. Evol. 13, 415–440 (1984).
Carvalho, S., Cunha, E., Sousa, C. & Matsuzawa, T. Chaînes opératoires and resource-exploitation strategies in chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) nut cracking. J. Hum. Evol. 55, 148–163 (2008).
Bain, M. et al. Automated audiovisual behavior recognition in wild primates. Sci. Adv. 7, eabi4883 (2021).
Biro, D., Sousa, C. & Matsuzawa, T. in Cognitive Development in Chimpanzees (eds Matsuzawa, T. et al.) 476–506 (Springer, 2006).
Biro, D. et al. Cultural innovation and transmission of tool use in wild chimpanzees: evidence from field experiments. Anim. Cogn. 6, 213–223 (2003).
Inoue-Nakamura, N. & Matsuzawa, T. Development of stone tool use by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J. Comp. Psychol. 111, 159–173 (1997).
Neufuss, J., Humle, T., Cremaschi, A. & Kivell, T. L. Nut-cracking behaviour in wild-born, rehabilitated bonobos (Pan paniscus): a comprehensive study of hand-preference, hand grips and efficiency. Am. J. Primatol. 79, e22589 (2017).
Howard-Spink, E. et al. Old age variably impacts chimpanzee engagement and efficiency in stone tool use. Preprint at https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2024.11.25.625128v1 (2024).
Luncz, L. V. et al. Tool skill impacts the archaeological evidence across technological primates. Sci. Rep. 14, 16556 (2024).
Humle, T., Snowdon, C. T. & Matsuzawa, T. Social influences on ant-dipping acquisition in the wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) of Bossou, Guinea, West Africa. Anim. Cogn. 12, S37–S48 (2009).
Lonsdorf, E. V., Eberly, L. E. & Pusey, A. E. Sex differences in learning in chimpanzees. Nat. Br. Commun. 428, 715–716 (2004).
Schofield, D., McGrew, W. C., Takahashi, A. & Hirata, S. Cumulative culture in nonhumans: overlooked findings from Japanese monkeys? Primates 59, 113–122 (2018).
Carvalho, S., Biro, D., McGrew, W. C. & Matsuzawa, T. Tool-composite reuse in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): archaeologically invisible steps in the technological evolution of early hominins? Anim. Cogn. 12, 103–114 (2009).
Sirianni, G., Luncz, L. V. & Gratton, P. in The Chimpanzees of the Taï Forest: 40 Years of Research (eds Boesch, C. & Wittig, R.) 104–124 (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2019).
Schrauf, C., Call, J., Fuwa, K. & Hirata, S. Do chimpanzees use weight to select hammer tools? PLoS ONE 7, 1–12 (2012).
Matsuzawa, T. in Chimpanzee Cultures (eds Wrangham, R. W. et al.) 351–370 (Harvard Univ. Press, 1994).
Koops, K., Biro, D., Matsuzawa, T., McGrew, W. C. & Carvalho, S. Appropriate knowledge of wild chimpanzee behavior (‘know-what’) and field experimental protocols (‘know-how’) are essential prerequisites for testing the origins and spread of technological behavior. Response to ‘Unmotivated subjects cannot provide interpretable data and tasks with sensitive learning periods require appropriately aged subjects’ by C. Tennie and J. Call. Anim. Behav. Cogn. 10, 163–168 (2023).
Gruber, T., Clay, Z. & Zuberbühler, K. A comparison of bonobo and chimpanzee tool use: evidence for a female bias in the Pan lineage. Anim. Behav. 80, 1023–1033 (2010).
Chapman, C. A., Corriveau, A., Schoof, V. A. M., Twinomugisha, D. & Valenta, K. Long-term simian research sites: significance for theory and conservation. J. Mammal. 98, 652–660 (2017).
Matsuzawa, T. in The Chimpanzees of Bossou and Nimba (eds Matsuzawa, T. et al.) 157–164 (Springer, 2011).
Koops, K., Gaspard Soumah, A., van Leeuwen, K. L., Didier Camara, H. & Matsuzawa, T. Field experiments find no evidence that chimpanzee nut cracking can be independently innovated. Nat. Hum. Behav. 6, 487–494 (2022).
Humle, T. in The Chimpanzees of Bossou and Nimba (eds Matsuzawa, T. et al.) 13–21 (Springer, 2011).
Sugiyama, Y. & Koman, J. Social structure and dynamics of wild chimpanzees at Bossou, Guinea. Primates 20, 323–339 (1979).
Matsuzawa, T. & Humle, T. in The Chimpanzees of Bossou and Nimba (eds Matsuzawa, T. et al.) 3–10 (Springer, 2011).
Sugiyama, Y. & Koman, J. Tool-using and -making behaviour in wild chimpanzees at Bossou, Guinea. Primates 20, 513–524 (1979).
Sugiyama, Y., Fushimi, T., Sakura, O. & Matsuzawa, T. Hand preference and tool use in wild chimpanzees. Primates 34, 151–159 (1993).
Friard, O. & Gamba, M. BORIS: a free, versatile open-source event-logging software for video/audio coding and live observations. Methods Ecol. Evol. 7, 1325–1330 (2016).
R Core Team R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2023); https://www.R-project.org
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B. & Walker, S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67, 1–48 (2015).
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B. & Christensen, R. H. B. lmerTest package: tests in linear mixed effects models. J. Stat. Softw. 82, 1–26 (2017).
Field, A., Miles, J. & Field, Z. Discovering Statistics Using R (SAGE, 2012).
Brooks, M. E. et al. glmmTMB balances speed and flexibility among packages for zero-inflated generalized linear mixed modeling. R J. 9, 378–400 (2017).
Lüdecke, D., Ben-Shachar, M. S., Patil, I. & Makowski, D. performance: an R package for assessment, comparison and testing of statistical models. J. Open Source Softw. 6, 3139 (2021).
Christensen, R. H. B. ordinal—regression models for ordinal data. R package version 2019.12-10 (2019); https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=ordinal
Koo, T. K. & Li, M. Y. A guideline of selecting and reporting intraclass correlation coefficients for reliability research. J. Chiropr. Med. 15, 155–163 (2016).
Acknowledgements
We thank the Direction Générale de la Recherche Scientifique et de l’Innovation Technologique (DGERSIT) and the Institut de Recherche Environnementale de Bossou (IREB) in Guinea for research authorization. The original video archive was digitized, organized and systematized by D. Schofield. Special thanks to all the field researchers and assistants at Bossou, specifically H. D. Camara, C. V. Mami, G. Zogbila, B. Zogbila, J. Doré, G. Goumy, T. Camara, P. Goumy, P. Cherif, J. G. Doré and M. Doré. We thank all the researchers who contributed to the Bossou video archive since its inception, especially D. Biro, M. Hayashi and C. Hobaiter. We thank A. Mielke for help in cataloguing the Bossou video archive. We thank D. Biro, K. Almeida-Warren and K. Koops for feedback. We thank Z. Dai and V. Martignac for help with independent, hypothesis-blind coding for the inter-rater reliability analyses. This research was supported by the University of Oxford’s Clarendon Fund Scholarship (SFF1920_CB2_SSD_1153789) and the Boise Trust Fund (University of Oxford, UK) to S.B., a Junior Research Fellowship (Wadham College, University of Oxford) to A.J.D., and by the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology (MEXT), Japan (grant numbers 07102010, 12002009, 16002001, 20002001, 24000001 and 16H06283), to T.M. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the paper.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
S.B. conceived of the study, designed and coordinated the study, collected data, analysed and visualized the data, and wrote the paper. E.C. provided supervision, participated in the design of the study and commented on the paper. A.J.D. analysed and visualized the data and commented on the paper. T.M. collected the original dataset and commented on the paper. S.C. collected the original dataset, provided supervision, participated in the design of the study and commented on the paper.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Peer review
Peer review information
Nature Human Behaviour thanks Jean-Baptiste Leca, Lydia Luncz and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Peer reviewer reports are available.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Supplementary Information
Supplementary Figs. 1–7, discussion and Tables 1–7.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Berdugo, S., Cohen, E., Davis, A.J. et al. Reliable long-term individual variation in wild chimpanzee technological efficiency. Nat Hum Behav 9, 472–480 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-024-02071-8
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-024-02071-8