Abstract
Growing evidence indicates that migratory animals exploit the magnetic field of the Earth for navigation, both as a compass to determine direction and as a map to determine geographical position1. It has long been proposed that, to navigate using a magnetic map, animals must learn the magnetic coordinates of the destination2,3, yet the pivotal hypothesis that animals can learn magnetic signatures of geographical areas has, to our knowledge, yet to be tested. Here we report that an iconic navigating species, the loggerhead turtle (Caretta caretta), can learn such information. When fed repeatedly in magnetic fields replicating those that exist in particular oceanic locations, juvenile turtles learned to distinguish magnetic fields in which they encountered food from magnetic fields that exist elsewhere, an ability that might underlie foraging site fidelity. Conditioned responses in this new magnetic map assay were unaffected by radiofrequency oscillating magnetic fields, a treatment expected to disrupt radical-pair-based chemical magnetoreception4,5,6, suggesting that the magnetic map sense of the turtle does not rely on this mechanism. By contrast, orientation behaviour that required use of the magnetic compass was disrupted by radiofrequency oscillating magnetic fields. The findings provide evidence that two different mechanisms of magnetoreception underlie the magnetic map and magnetic compass in sea turtles.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution
Access options
Access Nature and 54 other Nature Portfolio journals
Get Nature+, our best-value online-access subscription
27,99 € / 30 days
cancel any time
Subscribe to this journal
Receive 51 print issues and online access
199,00 € per year
only 3,90 € per issue
Buy this article
- Purchase on SpringerLink
- Instant access to full article PDF
Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout





Similar content being viewed by others
Data availability
There are no restrictions on data availability. Access to the data can be found on GitHub (https://github.com/kaylago/Goforthetal_LearnedMagneticMapCuesandTwoMechanismsofMagnetoreceptioninTurtles.git). Source data are provided with this paper.
Code availability
Custom-written software by A.H. facilitated data collection in the compass and map assay experiments, but the software was not central to the research or conclusions. The code can be accessed on GitHub (https://github.com/radiotech/Caretta2_Encoder).
References
Lohmann, K. J., Goforth, K. M., Mackiewicz, A. G., Lim, D. S. & Lohmann, C. M. F. Magnetic maps in animal navigation. J. Comp. Physiol. A 208, 41–67 (2022).
Gould, J. L. Animal navigation: memories of home. Curr. Biol. 25, R104–R106 (2015).
Gould, J. L. & Gould, C. G. Nature’s Compass (Princeton Univ. Press, 2012).
Ritz, T., Thalau, P., Phillips, J. B., Wiltschko, R. & Wiltschko, W. Resonance effects indicate a radical-pair mechanism for avian magnetic compass. Nature 429, 177–180 (2004).
Henbest, K. B., Kukura, P., Rodgers, C. T., Hore, P. J. & Timmel, C. R. Radio frequency magnetic field effects on a radical recombination reaction: a diagnostic test for the radical pair mechanism. J. Am. Chem. Soc. 126, 8102–8103 (2004).
Granger, J., Cummer, S. A., Lohmann, K. J. & Johnsen, S. Environmental sources of radio frequency noise: potential impacts on magnetoreception. J. Comp. Physiol. A 208, 83–95 (2022).
Alerstam, T. & Bäckman, J. Ecology of animal migration. Curr. Biol. 28, R968–R972 (2018).
Putman, N. Marine migrations. Curr. Biol. 28, R972–R976 (2018).
Warrant, E. et al. The Australian bogong moth Agrotis infusa: a long-distance nocturnal navigator. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 10, 77 (2016).
Wiltschko, R. & Wiltschko, W. The discovery of the use of magnetic navigational information. J. Comp. Physiol. A 208, 9–18 (2021).
Lohmann, K. J., Lohmann, C. M. F., Ehrhart, L. M., Bagley, D. A. & Swing, T. Geomagnetic map used in sea turtle navigation. Nature 428, 909–910 (2004).
Brothers, J. R. & Lohmann, K. J. Evidence for geomagnetic imprinting and magnetic navigation in the natal homing of sea turtles. Curr. Biol. 25, 392–396 (2015).
Wynn, J., Padget, O., Mouritsen, H., Perrins, C. & Guilford, T. Natal imprinting to the Earth’s magnetic field in a pelagic seabird. Curr. Biol. 30, 2869–2873.e2 (2020).
Lohmann, K. J., Cain, S. D., Dodge, S. A. & Lohmann, C. M. Regional magnetic fields as navigational markers for sea turtles. Science 294, 364–366 (2001).
Lohmann, K. J., Putman, N. F. & Lohmann, C. M. F. The magnetic map of hatchling loggerhead sea turtles. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 22, 336–342 (2012).
Musick, J. A. & Limpus, C. J. in The Biology of Sea Turtles Vol. 1 (eds Lutz, P. L. & Musick, J. A.) 137–163 (CRC, 1997).
Avens, L., Braun-McNeill, J., Epperly, S. & Lohmann, K. J. Site fidelity and homing behavior in juvenile loggerhead sea turtles (Caretta caretta). Mar. Biol. 143, 211–220 (2003).
Broderick, A. C., Coyne, M. S., Fuller, W. J., Glen, F. & Godley, B. J. Fidelity and over-wintering of sea turtles. Proc. R. Soc. B 274, 1533–1539 (2007).
Putman, N. F., Endres, C. S., Lohmann, C. M. F. & Lohmann, K. J. Longitude perception and bicoordinate magnetic maps in sea turtles. Curr. Biol. 21, 463–466 (2011).
Putman, N. F. et al. An inherited magnetic map guides ocean navigation in juvenile Pacific salmon. Curr. Biol. 24, 446–450 (2014).
Lohmann, K. J., Hester, J. T. & Lohmann, C. M. F. Long-distance navigation in sea turtles. Ethol. Ecol. Evol. 11, 1–23 (1999).
Gaspar, P. et al. Oceanic dispersal of juvenile leatherback turtles: going beyond passive drift modeling. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 457, 265–284 (2012).
Scott, R., Marsh, R. & Hays, G. C. Ontogeny of long distance migration. Ecology 95, 2840–2850 (2014).
Avens, L. & Lohmann, K. J. Navigation and seasonal migratory orientation in juvenile sea turtles. J. Exp. Biol. 207, 1771–1778 (2004).
González Carman, V. et al. Revisiting the ontogenetic shift paradigm: the case of juvenile green turtles in the SW Atlantic. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 429, 64–72 (2012).
Meylan, P. A., Hardy, R. F., Gray, J. A. & Meylan, A. B. A half-century of demographic changes in a green turtle (Chelonia mydas) foraging aggregation during an era of seagrass decline. Mar. Biol. 169, 74 (2022).
Putman, N. F. & Lohmann, K. J. Compatibility of magnetic imprinting and secular variation. Curr. Biol. 18, R596–R597 (2008).
Lohmann, K. J., Lohmann, C. M. F. & Endres, C. S. The sensory ecology of ocean navigation. J. Exp. Biol. 211, 1719–1728 (2008).
Dacke, M. et al. Multimodal cue integration in the dung beetle compass. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 116, 14248–14253 (2019).
Johnsen, S. & Lohmann, K. J. The physics and neurobiology of magnetoreception. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 703–712 (2005).
Nordmann, G. C., Hochstoeger, T. & Keays, D. A. Magnetoreception — a sense without a receptor. PLoS Biol. 15, e2003234 (2017).
Ritz, T., Adem, S. & Schulten, K. A model for photoreceptor-based magnetoreception in birds. Biophys. J. 78, 707–718 (2000).
Hore, P. J. & Mouritsen, H. The radical-pair mechanism of magnetoreception. Annu. Rev. Biophys. 45, 299–344 (2016).
Maeda, K. et al. Magnetically sensitive light-induced reactions in cryptochrome are consistent with its proposed role as a magnetoreceptor. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 109, 4774–4779 (2012).
Wan, G., Hayden, A. N., Iiams, S. E. & Merlin, C. Cryptochrome 1 mediates light-dependent inclination magnetosensing in monarch butterflies. Nat. Commun. 12, 771 (2021).
Xu, J. et al. Magnetic sensitivity of cryptochrome 4 from a migratory songbird. Nature 594, 535–540 (2021).
Bradlaugh, A. A. et al. Essential elements of radical pair magnetosensitivity in Drosophila. Nature 615, 111–116 (2023).
Nimpf, S. & Keays, D. A. Myths in magnetosensation. iScience 25, 104454 (2022).
Quinn, T. P., Merrill, R. T. & Brannon, E. L. Magnetic field detection in sockeye salmon. J. Exp. Zool. 217, 137–142 (1981).
Lohmann, K. J. & Lohmann, C. M. F. A light-independent magnetic compass in the leatherback sea turtle. Biol. Bull. 185, 149–151 (1993).
Muheim, R., Sjöberg, S. & Pinzon-Rodriguez, A. Polarized light modulates light-dependent magnetic compass orientation in birds. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 113, 1654–1659 (2016).
Netušil, R. et al. Cryptochrome-dependent magnetoreception in a heteropteran insect continues even after 24 h in darkness. J. Exp. Biol. 224, jeb243000 (2021).
Wiltschko, R., Ahmad, M., Nießner, C., Gehring, D. & Wiltschko, W. Light-dependent magnetoreception in birds: the crucial step occurs in the dark. J. R. Soc. Interface 13, 20151010 (2016).
Light, P., Salmon, M. & Lohmann, K. J. Geomagnetic orientation of loggerhead sea turtles: evidence for an inclination compass. J. Exp. Biol. 182, 1–10 (1993).
Kirschvink, J. L., Walker, M. M. & Diebel, C. E. Magnetite-based magnetoreception. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 11, 462–467 (2001).
Irwin, W. P. & Lohmann, K. J. Disruption of magnetic orientation in hatchling loggerhead sea turtles by pulsed magnetic fields. J. Comp. Physiol. A 191, 475–480 (2005).
Munro, U., Munro, J. A., Phillips, J. B. & Wiltschko, W. Effect of wavelength of light and pulse magnetisation on different magnetoreception systems in a migratory bird. Aust. J. Zool. 45, 189–198 (1997).
Wiltschko, W. & Wiltschko, R. Migratory orientation of European robins is affected by the wavelength of light as well as by a magnetic pulse. J. Comp. Physiol. A 177, 363–369 (1995).
Qin, S. et al. A magnetic protein biocompass. Nat. Mater. 15, 217–226 (2016).
Xie, C. Searching for unity in diversity of animal magnetoreception: from biology to quantum mechanics and back. Innovation 3, 100229 (2022).
Meister, M. Physical limits to magnetogenetics. eLife 5, e17210 (2016).
Kalmijn, A. J. in Handbook of Sensory Physiology Vol. 3 (ed. Fessard, A.) 147–200 (Springer-Verlag, 1974).
Nimpf, S. et al. A putative mechanism for magnetoreception by electromagnetic induction in the pigeon inner ear. Curr. Biol. 29, 4052–4059.e4 (2019).
Phillips, J. B. & Diego-Rasilla, F. J. The amphibian magnetic sense(s). J. Comp. Physiol. A 208, 723–742 (2022).
Wiltschko, R. et al. Magnetoreception in birds: the effect of radio-frequency fields. J. R. Soc. Interface 12, 20141103 (2015).
Szabo, B., Noble, D. W. A. & Whiting, M. J. Learning in non-avian reptiles 40 years on: advances and promising new directions. Biol. Rev. 96, 331–356 (2021).
Alldred, J. C. & Scollar, I. Square cross section coils for the production of uniform magnetic fields. J. Sci. Instrum. 44, 755–760 (1967).
Lohmann, K. & Lohmann, C. Detection of magnetic inclination angle by sea turtles: a possible mechanism for determining latitude. J. Exp. Biol. 194, 23–32 (1994).
Lohmann, K. J. & Lohmann, C. M. F. Detection of magnetic field intensity by sea turtles. Nature 380, 59–61 (1996).
Endres, C. S., Putman, N. F. & Lohmann, K. J. Perception of airborne odors by loggerhead sea turtles. J. Exp. Biol. 212, 3823–3827 (2009).
Friard, O. & Gamba, M. BORIS: a free, versatile open-source event-logging software for video/audio coding and live observations. Methods Ecol. Evol. 7, 1325–1330 (2016).
R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. http://www.R-project.org/ (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2013).
Kassambara, A. rstatix: Pipe-friendly framework for basic statistical tests. R version 0.7.2 https://cran.r-project.org/package=rstatix (2023).
Anderson, D. esvis: Visualization and estimation of effect sizes. R version 0.3.1 https://cran.r-project.org/package=esvis (2020).
Lüdecke, D. esc: Effect size computation for meta analysis. R version 0.5.1 https://cran.r-project.org/package=esc (2019).
Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., DebRoy, S. & Sarkar, D. nlme: Linear and nonlinear mixed effects models. R version 3.1-166 https://cran.r-project.org/package=nlme (2019).
Caldwell, A. R. Exploring equivalence testing with the updated TOSTER R package. Preprint at PsyArXiv https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/ty8de (2022).
Lakens, D. Equivalence tests: a practical primer for t tests, correlations, and meta-analyses. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 8, 355–362 (2017).
Wickham, H. ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis (Springer-Verlag, 2016).
Massicotte, P. & South, A. rnaturalearth: World map data from natural earth. R version 1.0.1.9000 https://docs.ropensci.org/rnaturalearth/ (2023).
Pebesma, E. Simple Features for R: standardized support for spatial vector data. R J. 10, 439–446 (2018).
Pebesma, E. & Bivand, R. Spatial Data Science: With Applications in R (Chapman and Hall/CRC, 2023).
Putman, N. F., Verley, P., Endres, C. S. & Lohmann, K. J. Magnetic navigation behavior and the oceanic ecology of young loggerhead sea turtles. J. Exp. Biol. 218, 1044–1050 (2015).
Merritt, R., Purcell, C. & Stroink, G. Uniform magnetic field produced by three, four, and five square coils. Rev. Sci. Instrum. 54, 879–882 (1983).
Lohmann, K. J. Magnetic orientation by hatchling loggerhead sea turtles (Caretta caretta). J. Exp. Biol. 155, 37–49 (1991).
Agostinelli, C. & Lund, U. R package “circular”: circular statistics. R version 0.5-1 https://cran.r-project.org/package=circular (2022).
COMSOL AB. COMSOL Multiphysics® (COMSOL AB, 2022).
Harris, C. R. et al. Array programming with NumPy. Nature 585, 357–362 (2020).
Acknowledgements
We thank J. R. Brothers, D. Ernst, S. Johnsen, J. Granger, D. Steinberg, L. Naisbett-Jones, H. Havens and A. Mackiewicz for discussions of experimental design and manuscript drafts; I. Wilson, A. Barnett, J. Settelen, M. Hankins, A. Boyce, C. Manzonelli, N. Bal, N. Iverson, A. Jacks, E. Tsai, K. Steininger, D. Oakley, K. Ritterpusch, W. Jian, V. Sirupirapu, I. Taylor, G. Knaack, S. Hu, E. Cho, M. Kennedy, A. Halferty, C. Koricke, C. Herschfield, A. Ronn, I. Donnolo, V. Zhang, L. Johnson, A. Kang, T. Hinton, M. Kane, R. Davids, S. Palmieri, L. Prince, S. Nichols, W. Hammond, S. Gerber and A. Byrd for assistance with conditioning and data analyses; L. Soltan, G. Sollom, M. Babb and W. Gary for help with turtle care; L. Prince for assistance with nest marking; and A. Goforth, D. Goforth and A. Wasif for assistance with coil construction and transport.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
The research plan was conceived by K.M.G., K.J.L. and C.M.F.L. K.M.G. conducted the conditioning experiments and data analyses with input from K.J.L. and C.M.F.L. K.M.G., D.S.L., T.L.H., K.J.L. and C.M.F.L. conducted the compass and map assay experiments. A.H. wrote the software used in some experiments. A.G. and R.H. generated and measured the radiofrequency fields. K.M.G., K.J.L. and C.M.F.L. drafted the manuscript, which was revised with input from all authors. This research was supported by the Air Force Office of Scientific Research grant FA9550-20-1-0399 to K.J.L. and the National Science Foundation grant IOS-1456923 to K.J.L. and C.M.F.L. A.G. was supported by the National Science Foundation grant OISE-1743790. R.H. and A.G. were supported by the US Department of Energy, Office of Science, Office of Nuclear Physics grants DEFG02-97ER41041 and DEFG02- 97ER41033.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Peer review
Peer review information
Nature thanks Basil el Jundi, Guijun Wan and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Extended data figures and tables
Extended Data Fig. 1 Results from Fig. 1 plotted on a linear scale.
Turtles learned to discriminate between magnetic fields replicating ones that exist near New Hampshire, U.S.A. and in the Gulf of Mexico. (a) Map showing relative locations of the two treatment fields. The map was created using Natural Earth (https://www.naturalearthdata.com; credit Tom Patterson and Nathaniel Vaughn Kelso). (b) In tests conducted immediately after the conditioning period, turtles exhibited significantly higher levels of turtle dance behavior when experiencing the field in which they had been fed (two-tailed Wilcoxon signed-rank test, w = 123, p = 0.003, Hedge’s g = 0.88, n = 16, ● = turtles rewarded in the New Hampshire field, ▲ = turtles rewarded in the Gulf of Mexico field). See Methods for details of analysis. (c) Turtles were tested a second time, four months after the initial experiments, without experiencing either the rewarded or unrewarded field in the interim. Turtles still discriminated between the two fields (two-tailed Wilcoxon signed-rank test, w = 118, p = 0.01, Hedge’s g = 0.62, n = 16). Error bars represent standard error.
Extended Data Fig. 2 Results of additional map assay experiments plotted on a linear scale.
In four additional experiments, turtles discriminated between a magnetic field in which they were fed and one in which they were not. Turtles differentiated between magnetic fields that exist near: (a) Delaware, U.S.A. and Cuba (two-tailed Wilcoxon signed-rank test, w = 108, p = 0.04, Hedge’s g = 0.50, n = 16); (b) Maine and Florida, U.S.A. (two-tailed Wilcoxon signed-rank test, w = 121, p = 0.004, Hedge’s g = 0.63, n = 16); (c) Newfoundland, Canada and Virginia, U.S.A. (two-tailed Wilcoxon signed-rank test, w = 115, p = 0.01, Hedge’s g = 0.60, n = 16); and (d) the Turks and Caicos Islands and Haiti (two-tailed Wilcoxon signed-rank test, w = 97, p = 0.003, Hedge’s g = 0.60, n = 14). The data in (c) represent a second conditioning experiment conducted with the same turtles used in (b) and thus indicate that turtles can learn magnetic fields that exist at multiple locations. For each pair of magnetic fields, the rewarded field for the turtle is indicated by either ● or ▲ as indicated on the figure. Remaining conventions as in Fig. 1. The maps were created using Natural Earth (https://www.naturalearthdata.com; credit Tom Patterson and Nathaniel Vaughn Kelso).
Extended Data Fig. 3 Data from all map assay experiments (Fig. 3a) plotted on a linear scale.
Turtles learned to discriminate between a magnetic field in which they received food and one in which they did not (two-tailed Wilcoxon signed-rank test, w = 2676, p = 1.6 × 10−8, Hedge’s g = 0.50, n = 78). Conventions as in Fig. 3a.
Extended Data Fig. 4 Percent change in turtle dancing responses for all turtles.
Percent change = \(\frac{{Rewarded\; field\; turtle\; dancing}-{Unrewarded\; field\; turtle\; dancing}}{{Unrewarded\; field\; turtle\; dancing}}\,* \,100\). Red dotted lines indicate 0% change relative to the unrewarded field. Dots represent the percent change for individuals; dot color corresponds to the rewarded magnetic field as indicated on the figure. All data were analyzed with one-tailed Wilcoxon signed-rank tests. (a) Turtles conditioned to magnetic fields near New Hampshire, U.S.A. and the Gulf of Mexico had a percent change in dancing behavior significantly greater than zero (w = 127, p = 0.0005, Hedge’s g = 0.80, n = 16). (b) When these same turtles were tested four months after conditioning ended, without exposure to either field in the interim, percent change in dancing was again significantly greater than zero (w = 124, p = 0.001, Hedge’s g = 0.85, n = 16). (c) Turtles conditioned to Delaware, U.S.A. and Cuba had a percent change in dancing behavior significantly greater than zero (w = 118, p = 0.004, Hedge’s g = 0.45, n = 16). (d) Turtles with a rewarded field of Maine, U.S.A. had a percent change in dancing significantly greater than zero (w = 121, p = 0.002, Hedge’s g = 0.80, n = 16). (e) Turtles conditioned to Newfoundland, Canada and Virginia, U.S.A., had a percent change in dancing significantly greater than zero (w = 120, p = 0.003, Hedge’s g = 0.63, n = 16). (f) Turtles conditioned to Haiti and the Turks and Caicos had a percent change in dancing significantly greater than zero (w = 99, p = 0.0009, Hedge’s g = 0.94, n = 14). Collectively, these analyses of percent change corroborate the findings based on raw data in Figs. 1 and 2.
Extended Data Fig. 5 Measured RF magnetic flux at water level in the testing environments in the two RF experiments.
(a) Magnetic flux density of the broadband oscillating magnetic fields, as well as the background magnetic field fluctuations, produced during the map assay experiments. Single runs (a single measurement of the field) are displayed as vertical dashed lines and are digitized with 25 MHz sampling frequency and a 2048 sample buffer. Measurements extended to 12 MHz but only values in the targeted range (0.1–10 MHz) were included in calculations. (b) Magnetic flux density of the broadband magnetic fields produced and the background magnetic field fluctuations during the compass & map assay experiments. Single runs are displayed as vertical dashed lines and are digitized with 20 MHz sampling frequency and a 1200 sample buffer. In (a) and (b) solid lines represent the average magnetic noise density, calculated from 10 and 8 repeated measurements respectively.
Supplementary information
Supplementary Video 1
Turtle dance behavior. The video shows one sequence of vigorous turtle dance behavior in the presence of food, followed by several more subtle instances of turtle dance behavior when a turtle was in the rewarded magnetic field but food was absent. Turtles swimming in the unrewarded field are also shown to illustrate baseline behavior for comparative purposes.
Rights and permissions
Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.
About this article
Cite this article
Goforth, K.M., Lohmann, C.M.F., Gavin, A. et al. Learned magnetic map cues and two mechanisms of magnetoreception in turtles. Nature 638, 1015–1022 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-024-08554-y
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-024-08554-y
This article is cited by
-
Migratory moths navigate using the stars
Nature (2025)